Working knowledge



Download 1.09 Mb.
Page32/35
Date06.08.2017
Size1.09 Mb.
#27884
1   ...   27   28   29   30   31   32   33   34   35

The successfully predictive theories of human behavior begin with humans as evolved primates. Darwinian selection has given us certain innate predispositions. Those that govern emotion and information processing—cognition—are the most important, from a social theorist’s point of view.

Human cognition and human culture evolved for hunting and gathering, specifically in the East African landscape of mountains, high plains, vast lakes, rivers, and dry savannahs. We are adaptable enough to do well everywhere from cities to Arctic ice floes, but we should remember that our particular mix of emotion, reason, heuristics, and mental shortcuts evolved to dodge hyenas and lions while searching for seeds and berries.

Above all, it evolved to deal with sizable and complex social groups, often (if not usually) at war with similar neighboring ones. Our greatest joys and blessings are social, but by far our greatest curse—the true Original Sin—is our knee-jerk reflex to hate the neighboring group when anything goes wrong in our lives.

Predictive theories also begin with individuals, since only individual persons actually think and act. But they recognize that humans are always and necessarily social. Hobbes’ “savages” do not resemble anything that has existed anywhere in the primate order for 50 million years or so. Human persons develop through social interaction, and early learn the cultural rules and knowledge systems of their societies.

Social systems arising from interaction necessarily develop structure: kinship systems, hierarchy and leadership systems, economic structures, healing practices, religions (in some very broad sense, at least), and so on. All these originally develop to satisfy human wants and needs, but then take on a life of their own, as “emergents.”

Normal human information processing leads to countless mistakes. Most are in a systematic and predictable direction. People tend to attribute everything to deliberate agency until proven otherwise; religions are partly explained by this. People need to simplify this incredibly diverse and confusing world, and thus are prone to fall for any simplifying heuristic that is even remotely credible. This has proved the to be by far the deadliest error-maker in science. Occam’s Razor has slashed many a scientist’s career to ribbons.

One common reason for mistakes getting into cultures and nesting there is the natural tendency of people to go with the most simple and plausible explanation for a phenomenon, and use it till it is proved wrong. It almost always is, but by that time people have deduced from it a whole range of new “facts,” which take on a life of their own and persist indefinitely. We have observed this in the case of Galenic medicine. From the Four Elements came the Four Humors, and from those came countless explanations for ills and plans for curing them. The deductions were typically wrong. But, on the other hand, people constantly interpreted accurate empirical information in the light of Galenic medicine, and thus incorporated it into the system. Eventually, a huge system existed, full of wrong deductions and accurate but wrongly-explained observations. It was better than nothing, and thus lasted for two thousand years, until something clearly better came along (cf. Kuhn 1962).

The first and most obvious and human reaction to economic and social differences is to be hopeful and active during periods when things are getting better, passive when things level off, and increasingly angry, reactionary, and disaffected when things get worse. The devil in the details is our assessment of what “better” and “worse” mean in such situations. Long periods of sustained peace and economic progress bring out the best in people, long periods of decline bring out the worst, but the very best and very worst ares often brought out by sudden sharp changes. In the 1930s depression, Americans kept their faith and hope and revolutionized the country; Germans lost hope (partly because of their losses in World War I) and went to Hitler. In the current recession, Americans are losing hope and turning to fascism.

Knowing this psychological trait of humans is one of the few really effective ways of predicting the future.

Ordinary everyday knowledge is overwhelmingly of the useful kind. We know it because it is true and because we need it to live. It is thus best explained by functionalist theories. Dysfunctionalist theories work best in the political arena. They explain what we think we know about power, about competition, about our enemies.

Most knowledge is useful; it relates to the real world well enough to have value in our constant search for food, clothing, shelter, and above all the sociability which we crave above all other things. Therefore, most knowledge has developed through everyday interaction with people and things. It has not developed through formal logic, philosophical speculation, or divine inspiration. The vast majority of beliefs that we consider to be philosophy and religion develop from more ordinary, unstructured, everyday issues.

Humans are inveterate organizers, and Claude Lévi-Strauss was right to see that we structure everything in sight. He exaggerated how much we do this, but the postmodern tendency to see human thought as highly contingent, disorganized, culture-bound, and ad hoc is an even greater exaggeration in the other direction. If knowledge really were a random collection of culturally given beliefs, or a set of ideas created and propagated solely to maintain the power of an elite, we humans would never be able to find food or avoid predators or cure illness. On the other hand, if our thinking was locked into tight structures, we could not change with the times. In fact, we humans are extremely sensitive processors of information, and that information is derived from constant interaction with people and things. We test our ideas about the world, talk over what we find, test again, talk more, and eventually come up with working knowledge.

This should mean that we know more and more all the time, and on the whole this is true. But knowledge is distorted by closed feedback loops that drive error against clear sight. Elites are not the only ones who deploy shaky knowledge in self-serving ways. All the other ways of going wrong can lead to closed loops also.

People err in predictable ways. First, they must prioritize defense against threat, and they often imagine threats where only the possibility exists. So they are more defensive, resistant, and even aggressive than would be rational. Second, they follow their culture—more exactly, they go with the received wisdom in their reference groups, and usually follow their elders and peers. This makes sense—no one can start learning everything from scratch—but it must be checked by awareness of the limitations of received wisdom. This leads to the two most deadly and costly human mistakes: group hatreds and rigid top-down hierarchies. Third, humans naturally go with the most simple and immediate truths and inferences until these prove inadequate, and then work slowly toward wider and longer-range explanations. This is reasonable, but means that humans constantly over-discount the future and the wide context. They undercut the common good for small personal gains, which would seem like “selfishness” if they were not just as prone to undercut their own future good for trivial immediate pleasures.

Economic calculation may affect all our thinking (as economists insist), but it influences especially our ideas about money and goods, and so the theories based on rational individual choice are best at explaining that realm of culture. Politicians develop ideologies based on power and competition for it, so Weberian and Foucaultian theories do best in that area. Theories of interpersonal emotional interaction (like those in Stets and Turner 2006) do best at explaining love and hate. Darwinian theories seem to find their best use in explaining simple, basic strategies of mating and feeding. Thus, one must come up with a different mix of distorters and influences for every field of knowledge in a society.

All academics know that any social scientist is apt to believe that her field is the one with the real explanations. Economists believe in the universality of “economic man.” Anthropologists ridicule this, but are apt to think that “culture” can create preposterous levels of difference: people in Culture X are totally indifferent to wealth, or have no concept of mother love. Political scientists properly dismiss such views, but assume people live only to compete for power and position. Biologists assert everything is very heavily conditioned by evolved genetic mechanisms, in spite of the obvious changes in immigrant groups’ behavior within one generation.

The one truth that works perfectly for all cases is that all humans construct their knowledge from interaction—from actual, real-world, real-time dealings with people and things. Hardheaded economic calculus, wild passion, stark fear, social fictions and deceptions, and all manner of other types of influence all enter into the mind, mix and churn, and somehow create phenomenological experience. People share these experiences with each other—to an unknown degree, and by an imperfectly understood mechanism.

Culture is a useful shorthand term for what we learn from our wider society. Cultural knowledge is organized, with many models and systems embedded in a ground matrix of facts, customs, and ideas. But we have a less than perfect understanding of how cultural knowledge diffuses, or why people pick it up, or how they operate among the many complexities of scale, multiculturality, and rapid change.

Epistemology is not an easy discipline. We need to know much more about how we know.

For the future, tolerance, mutual aid, and valuing diversity are the basic and prime moral teachings. We are all in this together—not only seven billion humans divided into thousands of culturally defined groups, but also billions and billions of other organisms, on many of which we depend. With group hate as our greatest problem, and rejection of or indifference to the environment as a close second, we have to make conscious efforts to appreciate what we have.


References

Abram, David. 1996. The Spell of the Sensuous. New York: Pantheon.


Abu-Lughod, Lila. 1989. Veiled Sentiments: Honor and Poetry in a Bedouin Society. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Ahmad, S. Maqbul, and K. Baipakov. 2000. Geodesy, Geology and Mineralogy; Geography and Cartography; the Silk Route across Central Asia. In History of Civilizations of Central Asia, vol. IV, The Age of Achievement: A.D. 750 to the End of the Fifteenth Century. Part 2, The Achievements, edited by C. E. Bosworth and M. S. Asimov. Paris: UNESCO. Pp. 205-226.
Ahn, Junehui. 2010. The Myth of American Selfhood and Emotion: Raising a Sociocentric Child among Middle-Class Americans. Journal of Anthropological Research 66:378-396.
Alcázar, Baltasar del. 1961 [orig. 16th century]. “Preso de Amores.” In: Ocho Siglos de Poesía en lengua Castellana, Fancisco Montes de Oca, ed. Mexico City: Porrua. P. 167.
Alter, Adam L.; Virginia S. Y. Kwan. 2009. “Cultural Sharing in a Global Village: Evidence for Extracultural Cognition in European Americans.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96:742-760.
Alter, Adam L.; Daniel M. Oppenheimer; Jeffrey C. Zernia. 2010. “Missing the Trees for the Forest: A Construal Level Account of the Illusion of Explanatory Depth.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99:436-451.
Alvergne, Alexandra; Markus Jokela; Virpi Luumaa. 2010. “Personality and Reproductive Success in a High-Fertility Human Population.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107:11745-11750.
Anderson, Barbara A.; E. N. Anderson; Tracy Franklin; Aurora Dzib-Xihum de Cen. 2004. “Pathways of Decision Making among Yucatan Mayan Traditional Birth Attendants.” Journal of Midwifery and Women’s Health 49:4:312-319.
Anderson, Benedict. 1991. Imagined Communities. 2nd edn. London: Verso.
Anderson, E. N. 1972. "Some Chinese Methods of Dealing with Crowding." Urban Anthropology 1:2:141-150.
--- 1987. “Why is Humoral Medicine So Popular?" Social Science and Medicine 25:4:331-337.
--- 1988. The Food of China. New Haven: Yale University Press.
--- 1992. “Chinese Fisher Families: Variations on Chinese Themes.” Comparative Family Studies 23:2:231-247.
--- 1996. Ecologies of the Heart. New York: Oxford University Press.
--- 2005. Everyone Eats. New York: New York University Press.
--- ms1. The Morality of Ethnobiology. Ms. in prep.
--- ms 2. Learning from Experience.
--- 2006. Environment and Social Justice: Some Issues. Report to the administration, University of California, Riverside.
--- 2007. Floating World Lost. New Orleans: University Press of the South.
--- 2008. Mayaland Cuisine. Electronic publication, Lulu.com.
Anderson, E. N., and Marja Anderson. 1978. Fishing in Troubled Waters. Taipei: Orient Cultural Service.
Anderson, Perry. 1974. Lineages of the Absolutist State. London: NLB.
Andrade, Manuel J., and Hilaria Maas Collí. 1990. Cuentos Mayas Yucatecos. 2 v. Mérida: Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán.
Ankli, Anita; Otto Saticher; Michyale Heinrich. 1999a. “Medical Ethnobotany of the Yucatec Maya: Healers’ Consensus as a Quantitative Criterion.” Economic Botany 53:144-160.
--- 1999b. “Yucatec Maya Medicinal Plants Versus Nonmedicinal Plants: Indigenous Characterization and Selection.” Human Ecology 27:557-580.
Arellano Rodríguez, J. Alberto; José Salvador Flores Guido; Juan Tun Garrido; María Mercedes Cruz Bojórquez. 2003. Nomenclatura, forma de vida, uso, manejo y distribución de las especies vegetales de la Península de Yucatán. Mérida: Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán. Etnoflora Yucatanense no. 20.
Ariely, Dan. 2009. Predictably Irrational: The Hidden Forces That Shape Our Decisions. Rev. ed. New York: Harper.
Aristotle. 1952. Metaphysics. Tr. Richard Hope. New York: Columbia University Press.
--- 1955. The Ethics of Aristotle. Tr. J. A. K. Thomson. Harmondsworth, Sussex: Penguin.
Arrow, Holly. 2007. “The Sharp End of Altruism.” Science 318:581-582.
Ashton-Jones, Claire; William W. Maddux; Adam D. Galinsky; Tanya L. Chartrand. 2009. Who I Am Depends on How I Feel: The Role of Affect in the Expression of Culture. Psychological Science 20:340-346.
Atran, Scott. 1991. Cognitive Foundations of Natural History. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
--- 2002. In Gods We Trust. New York: Oxford University Press.
--- 2007. “Religion’s Social and Cognitive Landscape: An Evolutionary Perspective.” In Handbook of Cultural Psychology, Shinobu Kitayama and Dov Cohen, eds. New York: Guilford. Pp. 417-453.
--- 2010. Talking to the Enemy: Faith, Brotherhood, and the (Un)Making of Terrorists. New York: HarperCollins.
Azar, Beth. 2007. “All Puffed Up.” Monitor on Psychology, June, 15-16.
--- 2010a. “Revisiting Philosophy with fMRI.” Monitor on Psychology, Nov., pp. 40-43.
--- 2010b. “Your Brain on Culture.” Monitor on Psychology, Nov., 44-47.
Baker, Lee D. 1998. From Savage to Negro: Anthropology and the Construction of Race, 1896-1954. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Balam Pereira, Gilberto. 1992. Cosmogonia y uso actual de las plantas medicinales de Yucatán. Merida: Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán.
Bandura, Albert. 1982. “Self-Efficacy Mechanism in Human Agency.” American Psychologist 37:122-147.
--- 1986. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Barkow, Jeffrey; Leda Cosmides; John Tooby (eds.). 1992. The Adapted Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Barkun, Michael. 2003. A Culture of Conspiracy: Apocalyptic Visions in Contemporary America. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Barlett, Peggy. 1982. Agricultural Choice and Change. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Barlett, Peggy (ed.). 1980. Agricultural Decision Making. New York: Academic Press.
Barnett, Homer. 1953. Innovation: The Basis of Cultural Change. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Barthes, Roland. 1983. The Fashion System. Tr. Matthew Ward and Richard Howard. New York: Hill and Wang.
Bartlett, Jean. 1984. The Baha’i Faith, a Case Study in Adult Socialization. Ph.D. thesis, Dept. of Anthropology, University of California, Riverside.
Bartra, Roger. 1994. Savages in the Looking Glass. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Basu, Kaushik. 2007. “The Traveler’s Dilemma.” Scientific American, June, 90-95.
Bateson, Gregory. 1972. Steps to an Ecology of Mind: Collected Essays in Anthropology, Psychiatry, Evolution, and Epistemology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Baumeister, Roy F. 1997. Evil: Inside Human Violence and Cruelty. New York: Owl Books.
--- 2005. The Cultural Animal: Human Nature, Meaning, and Social Life. New York: Oxford University Press.
--- 2006. “Violent Pride: Do People Turn Violent Because of Self-Hate, or Self-Love?” Scientific American Mind, Aug.-Sept., 54-59.
Beals, Alan. 1967. Culture in Process. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Bean, Lowell John. 1972. Mukat’s People: The Cahuilla Indians of Southern California. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Beck, Ulrich. 1992. Risk Society: Toward a New Modernity. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Becker, Gary. 1996. Accounting for Tastes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Behrens, Timothy; Laurence T. Hunt; Mark W. Woolrich; Matthew F. S. Rushworth. 2008. “Associative Learning of Social Value.” Nature 456:245-249.

Bekoff, Marc. 2007. The Emotional Lives of Animals. Novato, CA: New World Library.


Bekoff, Marc, and Jessica Pierce. 2009. Wild Justice: The Moral Lives of Animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bellah, Robert. 1957. Tokugawa Religion: The Values of Pre-Industrial Japan. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Bellah, Robert; Richard Madsen; William M. Sullivan; Ann Swidler; Steven M. Tipton. 1996. Habits of the Heart: Individualism and Commitment in American Life. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Bellwood, Peter, and Colin Renfrew (eds.). 2002. Examining the Farming/Language Dispersal Hypothesis. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
Bennett, Bradley C. 2007. “Doctrine of Signatures: An Explanation of Medicinal Plant Disocvery or Dissemination of Knowledge?” Economic Botany 61:246-255.
Berger, Peter, and Thomas Luckmann. 1967. The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise of the Sociology of Knowledge. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Berlin, Brent. 1992. Ethnobiological Knowledge. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Berlin, Brent, and Paul Kay. 1969. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Bernal, Martin. 1987. Black Athena: The Afroasiatic Roots of Classical Civilization. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Bernhard, Helen; Urs Fischbacher; Ernst Fehr. 2006. “Parochial Altruism in Humans.” Nature 442:912-915.
Birkhead, Tim. 2008. The Wisodm of Birds: An Illustrated History of Ornithology. New York: Bloomsbury.

Bloom, Paul, and Deena Skolnick Weisberg. 2007 “Childhood Origins of Adult Resistance to Science.” Science 316:996-997.


Blaser, Mario. 2009. “The Threat of the Yrmo: The Political Ontology of a Sustainable Hunting Program.” American Anthropologist 111:10-20.
Blavascunas, Eunice. 2008. The Peasant and Communist Past in the Making of an Ecological Region: Podlasie, Poland. Ph.D. dissertation, Dept. of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Cruz.
Bloom, Paul. 2010. “How Do Morals Change?” Nature 464:490.
Blunt, Wilfrid. 1984. The Compleat Naturalist: A Life of Linnaeus. London: Collins.

Boas, Franz. 1995. A Wealth of Thought: Franz Boas on Native American Art. Ed/intro by Aldona Jonaitis. Seattle: University of Washington Press.


Boehm, Christopher. 1999. Equality in the Forest. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bond, Michael (ed.). 1986. The Psychology of the Chinese People. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977. Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
--- 1990. The Logic of Practice. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

--- 1991. The Political Ontology of Martin Heidegger. Tr. Peter Collier. Oxford: Polity Press. (French original, 1988.)


--- 1998. The State Nobility: Elite Schools in the Field of Power. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Bowker, Geoffrey, and Susan Leigh Star. 1999. Sorting Things Out: Classification and Its Consequences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bowles, Samuel. 2006. “Group Competition, Reproductive Leveling, and the Evolution of Human Altruism.” Science 314:1569-1572.
--- 2008. “Conflict: Altruism’s Midwife.” Nature 456:326-327.
--- 2009. Did Warfare among Ancestral Hunter-Gatherers Affect the Evolution of Human Social Behaviors?” Science 324:1293-1298.
Boyd, Robert. 2006. “The Puzzle of Human Sociality.” Science 314:1555-1556.
Boyd, Robert, and Peter Richerson. 2005. The Origin and Evolution of Cultures. New York: Oxford University Press.
Boyer, Pascal. 2001. Religion Explained: The Evolutionary Origins of Religious Thought. New York: Basic Books.
Breitenbach, Joana, and Pal Nyiri. 2009. Seeing Culture Everywhere, From Genocide to Consumer Habits. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Breslow, Sara Jo. 2011. Salmon Habitat Restoration, Farmland Preservation, and Environmental Drama in the Skagit River Valley. Ph.D. dissertation, Dept. of Anthropology, University of Washington.
Brindley, Erica Fox. 2010. Individualism in Early China: Human Agency and the Self in Thought and Politics. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.
Brown, Donald. 1991. Human Universals. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Brugger, Julie. 2009. Public Land and American Democratic Imaginaries: A Case Study of Conflict over the Management of Grand Staircase-Escalante National

Monument. Ph.D. dissertation, Dept. of Anthropology, University of Washington.


Bryner, Jeanna. 2009. “Health Care Debate Based on Total Lack of Logic.” Yahoo! News Online.
Buell, Paul; E. N. Anderson; Charles Perry. 2000. A Soup for the Qan. London: Kegan Paul International.
Burger, Jerry M. 2009. “Replicating Milgram: Would People Still Obey Today?” American Psychologist 64:1-11.
Burnett, D. Graham. 2007. Trying Leviathan. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Burton, Robert. 1932. The Anatomy of Melancholy. New York: E. P. Dutton. Orig. London, E. Cripps, 1651.
Buss, David. 2003. The Evolution of Desire. New York: Basic Books.
--- 2009. “The Great Struggles of Life.” American Psychologist 64:140-148.
Butler, David. 2008. Letter, Scientific American Mind, June/July, pp. 4-5.
Byrne, Richard, and Andrew Whiten 1988. Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intelligence in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cacioppo, John T.; James H. Fowler; Nicholas A. Christakis. 2009. “Alone in the Crowd: The Structure and Spread of Loneliness in a Large Social Network.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97:977-991.
Calne, Donald. 1999. With Reason: Rationality and Human Behavior. New York: Pantheon.
Camerer, Colin F., and Ernst Fehr. 2006. “When Does ‘Economic Man’ Dominate Social Behavior?” Science 311:47-52.
Cancian, Frank. 1979. The Innovator’s Situation: Upper-Middle-Class Conservatism in Agricultural Communties. Stanford.
Cao Xueqin (vols. 4-5 with Gao E). 1973-1986. The Story of the Stone. Translated by David Hawkes and John Minford. (Chinese original, 18th century.) New York: Penguin.
Caplan, Bryan. 2007. The Myth of the Rational Voter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Carpenter, Siri. 2008. “Buried Prejudice.” Scientific American Mind, April-May, 32-39.


Download 1.09 Mb.

Share with your friends:
1   ...   27   28   29   30   31   32   33   34   35




The database is protected by copyright ©ininet.org 2024
send message

    Main page