Arkansas Tech University The Culture Wars & Political Polarization in Perspective


KEY Same-sex partnerships Laws in the U.S



Download 4.38 Mb.
Page7/42
Date18.10.2016
Size4.38 Mb.
#2677
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   10   ...   42
KEY

Same-sex partnerships Laws in the U.S.

     Same-sex marriage

     Unions granting rights similar to marriage

     Legislation granting limited/enumerated rights

     Same-sex marriages performed elsewhere recognized

     No specific prohibition or recognition of same-sex marriages or unions

     Statute bans same-sex marriage

     Constitution bans same-sex marriage4

     Constitution bans same-sex marriage and some or all other kinds of same-sex unions


Massachusetts and California State Supreme Court decisions ruling gay marriage to be a constitutional right, legal elites have pushed gay marriage on to the public agenda and thus sparked a national debate, interest group activity, referendums, and counter-legislation.

Conflict and Consensus Models on Specific Gay Rights Issues, 1971-2007

Tables 5.2 and 5.3 report the regressions of the consensus measure, by issue type, on the time series in the GRD. It is important to note that each regression is of the time trend for the years in which that type of polling question was asked. So a null-result doesn’t necessarily mean that this issue hasn’t become more conflictual or more consensual over the last three decades. It merely means that there is no statistically significant trend over the years that it was asked in that time. This points to one of the problems with assessing trends in social (or any kind of issue) issues over any significant time period using polling and survey data. Many issues are only asked about in public opinion polls once they have slipped from consensus to conflict (and, thus, become a subject of political competition). This is almost certainly why contentious issues like gay marriage, civil unions, and the constitutional amendment have null findings in the trend models. For the years where we have data, those issues haven’t changed much. Civil Unions was a 54/42 split against in 2000. The constitutional amendment breakdown was 45/50 for in 2003, when it was first asked. So there is no apparent trend towards conflict in the regression models.

If, however, we had data on those questions for the entire time series, we would likely see a trend from consensus to conflict. There is a selection effect working against finding significant trend results for the more recent gay rights issues. If we don’t have data on the gay rights issue for the 1971-1985 period when the consensus on gay rights existed—nor the period of 1986 – 1992, when the big jump from conflict to consensus on gay rights occurs—then we are unlikely to find a trend either way. Not coincidentally, pollsters failed to ask about civil unions and constitutional amendments against gay marriage in the 1970’s. That these issues are the subject of contentions debate in the policy arena today is powerful evidence that attitudes towards gay rights have changed greatly over the last three decades.

Table 5.2: Conflict Trend Models for Gay Rights Issues 1971-2007

Model:

cm = b0 + b1(year) + e

Trend:

Conflict or

Consensus


Intercept

(S.E.)


Parameter Estimate

(S.E.)


R2

N

ISSUE CATEGORIES



















Gay Rights

Full Sample

CONF

79.310

(5.686)



-0.980

(0.206)


***

.032

685

Adoption


CONF


159.834

(28.396)


-4.0315

(0.999)


***

.414


25


Candidate


CONF

87.626

(20.305)



-1.572

(0.849)



*

.081

41

Friends


CONF

180.763

(41.376)


-4.897

(1.593)



***

.421

15

Morality


CONF

128.538

(22.119)



-2.946

(0.766)


***

.316

34

Vote


CONF

276.547

(25.068)



-7.519

(0.822)


***

.875

14

Homosexuals

-----

46.021

(0.579)



-0.078

(0.977)






.001

14


Movement


-----

92.657

(52.067)



-0.826

(2.077)





.011

17

Parties


-----

48.346

(27.238)



-0.638

(0.847)





.034

17

Civil Unions


-----

64.121

(37.791)



-1.260

(1.132)





.035

36

* significant at .10 level

** significant at .05 level

***significant at .01 level

Table 5.2 reports on the regression models of the consensus measure (CM) on the time-series that have negative coefficients, indicating that those issues have become more conflictual since the first year the pollsters began polling on them. The negative coefficient indicates a decline in consensus in the mass public on the issue. The “Gay Rights” model is for the entire GRD over the entire time-series. While the model fit is rather low (R2 =.032), there is statistically significant evidence that, on the whole, attitudes towards gay rights have moved from consensus to conflict. The regression coefficient indicates that gay rights attitudes have lost on average about a point on the consensus scale for each year of the time series. This result is substantively important, despite the low model-fitness statistic, when we consider how much of the deck is stacked against finding a statistically significant finding. As we know from Table 5.3, several of the gay rights issues move from conflict to consensus. Furthermore, several of the more conflictual gay rights issues were not collected over the full time-series and hence their almost certain movement from consensus to conflict is in the error term. So the fact that we find statistically significant findings for the full time-series is somewhat remarkable to begin with.

The largest movement from consensus to conflict is in the vote category, with respondents reporting more of a willingness to vote for a gay candidate by 7.52 points a year on average. The time-series independent variable accounts for nearly ninety percent of the variance in the model (.875). The consensus on gay adoption and respondents reporting they do not have gay friends has crumbled to the tune of about a four point loss in consensus each year. While it is possible the number of gays in society has increased exponentially, the more likely explanation is that gays are no more comfortable being ‘out’ in their social settings and heterosexuals are more willing to admit associations with homosexuals. Both of those models account for about 40 percent of the model variance. The consensus on the immorality of homosexual relations (-2.95) and on whether a candidate’s homosexuality is a public issue (-1.58) have also significantly declined, though the R2 for the candidate model is rather low (.081) in comparison to the other significant conflict models. The signs for the homosexual, gay rights movement, party, and civil unions are all in the conflict direction, but do not meet conventional standards of statistical significance. The conflict models in Table 5.2 indicate marked increase in conflict on gay rights as a whole and on a variety of fronts.

The story of gay rights attitudes in America is not exclusively a tale of conflict to consensus, as illustrated in Table 5.3. Table 5.3 reports on the gay rights issues which have moved from conflict to consensus over the course of the time-series, using the consensus measure (CM). There are three gay rights issues that have become more consensual since 1971. The largest shift towards consensus is in public attitudes towards gays in the military. There was always a rather obvious disconnect between opposition to gays in the military and the overwhelming percentage of Americans who believe that gays should have equal job opportunity and workplace rights. It is clear from the four point per year increase in consensus on gays in the military that the argument to treat the military differently than other kinds of jobs has not been persuasive. In May, 2007, seventy-eight percent of respondents asserted that openly gay individuals should be able to serve in the military. Despite this, no official change in the Don’t Ask, Don’t Tell policy has been adopted (though there have been a few rumblings along those lines). This may be more a case of policy priorities than a real reluctance to change the policy. On the other hand, a slight majority of Americans (52%) report support for DA,DT when presented with the alternative of openly gay soldiers or forbidding gay soldiers from serving entirely.34 As already noted previously, the American public has been supportive of equal opportunity in jobs for homosexuals from the beginning of the time-series in the early 1970’s. This has only increased over the last thirty-plus years, by about three points of consensus a year. The progress of time explains about forty percent of the variance in consensus for the model years. Lastly, a consensus has formed on comfort with homosexuals. This is



Table 5.3: Consensus Trend Models for Gay Rights Issues 1971-2007

Model:

cm = b0 + b1(year) + e

Trend:

Conflict or

Consensus


Intercept

(S.E.)


Parameter Estimate

(S.E.)


R2

N

ISSUE CATEGORIES



















Comfort


CONS

4.008

(24.764)



1.594

(0.917)


*

.144

20

Military


CONS

-63.660

(26.622)



4.002

(1.023)



***

.323

34

Jobs


CONS

51.523

(11.095)



3.008

(0.531)


***

.422

46

Legality


-----

23.624 (12.486)


0.451

(0.486)





.019

45

Teach


-----

14.763

(26.867)



1.820

(1.103)





.145

18

Constitution


-----

-104.871 (142.709)


4.724

(4.435)





.033

25

Benefits


-----

29.099 (46.733)

0.394

(1.647)






.003

20


Gay Marriage


-----


50.944

(18.570)



0.355

0.579





.003

105

* significant at .10 level

** significant at .05 level

***significant at .01 level

likely related to the increasing number of homosexuals who are portrayed positively in the media and on television. While Ellen as an openly gay sitcom character was highly controversial in the 1990’s, in a post Will & Grace world, a prominent gay leading character hardly merits comment in contemporary society.

Among the positive coefficient models that lack statistical significance, the model that is least likely to belong in this table is the constitutional amendment model. A public agenda issue at least as early as 2003, the American public has been evenly divided on the question for every year since. Opinion on the legality of homosexual relations, surprisingly, has not changed much over the course of the time-series despite data having been collected on it since 1977. The decision in Lawrence v. Texas is out-of-step with the majority of Americans. This is also the case with gay marriage, suggesting perhaps that public opinion has stabilized, if at a much more conflictual baseline than in the past.

Exploring the Path from Consensus to Conflict on Gay Rights

One of the methodological criticisms of the collapsed-category method of examining trends in polarization is that it—through the combination of strong and less-strong respondents—overstates the oppositional nature of public attitudes. First, this criticism is mostly inapplicable to the polling data set, as most of the questions have binary responses sets to begin with. However, one could argue those response sets themselves are too restrictive on opinions and attitudes in the American public. While flat distributions may be described as polarized using this method, it cannot affect polarization over time.

The fact that the use of a binary measure of public opinion (either through the original response set or collapsed categories) is consistent over the time period covered by the data set vitiates against a false positive on polarization due to the number of categories. I am looking at trends over time on these measures, and hence trends over these consistently coded variables cannot be a consequence of using a binary response measure (the number of categories is constant throughout the data set). Second, collapsing the categories and thus treating all the polls as if they had binary response sets permits one to compare polling questions with categorical response options that vary. So one must either sacrifice validity through collapsed measures or sacrifice coverage in restricting the analysis to just questions with the same number of categories in their response set. Furthermore, the binary measure accurately and consistently captures the distribution of opinion on gay rights as to conflict versus consensus. It permits the identification of conflict or consensus, the questions themselves permit variation in the centrality vs. extremism of the gay rights issue (ranging from forbidding speech and job opportunities to permitting gay marriage and providing social security benefits), and the characterization of debate on these issues in terms of two opposing viewpoints is a good proxy for the actual distribution of opinion in the mass public. Lastly, as illustrated in Table 5.4, the binary response measures may actually understate the degree of polarization in the public attitudes towards homosexual rights (note that the binary measures average about half of the variation in dispersion when compared to the four category measures).

In other words, it cuts both ways. While using a binary variable may overstate the polarization of a normally distributed variable or a relatively flat distribution, it would understate the polarization of an extreme bipolar distribution. That said, it is true that the use of a dichotomous measure may suggest polarization and conflict when the opinion distribution is relatively flat across a continuous measure. Thus the decision to use a dichotomous measure so as to include the maximum number and breadth of questions on gay rights represents a sacrifice in the validity of the measure. However, there remains an open question as to whether gay rights exhibits the flat distribution in opinion that would make the measure of consensus used here invalid. If gay rights opinion is not such a distribution, then this greatly mitigates the measurement problem. As such, I examine gay rights issues without collapsing categories by looking at a representative set of important gay rights issues from 2004 in Figures 5.3 reflecting the polarization on gay rights issues and a measure of the consensus that existed in the 1970’s in Figure 5.5. In Figure 5.4 I generate a hypothetical ‘normal’ distribution on gay rights issues to provide a baseline for comparison.




Figure 5.3: Polarized Attitudes on Gay Rights, 2004


Figure 5.3A: Gay Marriage Constitutional Amendment

Figure 5.3B: Gay Adoption



Figure 5.3C: Legality of Gay Marriage

Figure 5.3D: Gay Rights



Figure 5.4: Hypothetical “Central” Distribution on Gay Rights



Figure 5.5: Consensus Distribution on the Unacceptability of Homosexual Relationships, 1974



A Comparative Analysis of Gay Rights Attitude Polarization on Select Measures

Figure 5.3 reports four sets of responses to questions about contemporary gay rights issues from 2004: gay adoption, the legality of gay marriage, a constitutional amendment to define marriage as between a man and a woman (thus constitutionally barring gay marriage), and general attitudes towards gay rights (see Appendix C for exact question wording and poll information). Substantively speaking, these polls, ranging the breadth of contemporary gay rights issues, demonstrate that the American public has polarized on gay rights. All four of the distributions are bimodal or near-bimodal with more mass located in the extreme categories relative to the central categories (the extreme categories are also the two largest frequency categories for each of the issues). Figure 5.4 displays a hypothetical four-category centralized distribution with eighty-percent of the mass of public attitudes located in the two center categories. Figure 5.5 shows the consensus that existed on gay rights attitudes towards offspring being gay in the 1970’s. Over ninety-percent of respondents to the Virginia Slims poll reported that they would find their kid being gay unacceptable.35 The difference between the consensus against homosexual relations in contrast to the near-evenly divided and highly polarized distributions (conflict) in 2004 is plain.

Table 5.4 reports the univariate statistics on all four of the 2004 gay rights questions from Figures 5.4, 5.5, and 5.6. These statistics capture two important aspects of polarization: dispersion (standard deviation, coefficient of variation) and bimodality (kurtosis). Table 5.4 also includes the Virginia Slims question on the acceptability of homosexual relations for ones offspring from 1974, reflecting the consensus position against acceptability that existed at that time. For baseline comparison, I include the hypothetical near-normal four category variable with most of the mass of opinion located in the central categories (80% in center two categories, 20% in extreme categories) from Figure 5.4.

Table 5.4: Dispersion & Bimodality Statistics for Selected Gay Rights Polls & Hypothetical Distribution

Poll

Year

Mean

Standard Deviation

Kurtosis

Coefficient

of Variation



Gay Adoption

2004

2.783

1.232

-1.542

44.290

Constitutional Amendment

2004

2.467

1.323

-1.756

53.617

Gay Rights

2004

2.856

1.269

-1.508

44.423

Gay Marriage

2004

2.600

1.312

-1.743

50.492

Gay Adoption (binary)

2004

0.565

0.496

-1.934

87.793

Con. Amendment (binary)

2004

0.299

0.458

-1.223

153.261

Gay Rights (binary)

2004

0.463

0.499

-1.980

107.752

Gay Marriage (binary)

2004

0.482

0.500

-1.997

103.783

Homosexual Relationship

1974

0.979

0.143

43.186

14.591

Hypothetical

Centralized Distribution



----

2.5

0.807

-0.482

32.262

Table 5.4 points to the answers to several of the puzzles regarding polarization. It both illustrates the polarized nature of opinion on gay rights today vis-à-vis that in the 1970’s and provides a validity check for the polarization measures used in the aggregate analysis.36 Comparing the standard deviations of the Virginia Slims distribution, that of the hypothetical centralized distribution, and then the four gay rights issues from 2004 (4 cat), the linear increase in dispersion on the attribute is apparent. The standard deviations for the binary, collapsed opinions on gay rights from 2004 (0.500, 0.499, 0.496, and 0.458) are substantially larger than that of the opinion from 1974 (0.143). It is also the case with the coefficient of variation (14.591 for the 1974 poll; 32.262 for the hypothetical CD, and a 48.21 average COV for the 4 polls from 2004). The mean for the opinion on homosexual relationships from 1974 is near 1, indicating the near-consensus of opinion at that time while the means for the opinions from 2004 are near .5 (much more likely to indicate conflict).37

The kurtosis results are consistent with what we know about the included distributions and strong evidence of the increase in bimodality from the 1970’s to the 20o0’s. The four contemporaneous measures of public attitudes towards gay rights (four-cat or binary) all are bimodal, highly conflictual, and polarized distributions both from an absolute sense and in comparison to the opinion on gay rights prior to the onset of the culture war. Both the full and collapsed category measures of gay rights opinion in 2004 are platykurtic and hence indicate bimodality. The Virginia Slims poll on attitudes on homosexual relationships from 1974 reflects the consensus against homosexuals that existed at that time. The standard deviation for the 1974 poll is quite small (0.143) and small relative to the mean, as illustrated in the coefficient of variation (14.591). The kurtosis for this poll is off the charts leptokurtic (43.186), reflective of the single-peaked distribution and the near-total mass of responses located at it (no bimodality). Though the kurtosis measure inaccurately indicates slight bimodality in the centralized distribution, an ordinal comparison of the kurtosis results is consistent with the consensus-to-conflict expectation. It is thus helpful in looking at the relative peakedness differences between the distributions. Furthermore, note that the kurtosis of the distributions for both the two-category and the four-category versions of the polarized polling questions are relatively similar. This tends to weigh against Fiorina’s argument that the Abramowitz transformation (and, by implication, the transformation here) artificially inflates polarization, at least with respect to bimodality.

If we compare kurtosis relative to the other distributions, it illustrates the difference between a consensus position and a centralized distribution. The normal (or near-normal) distribution is not, as some skeptics treat it, the ultimate expression of consensus or moderation. This is tangible evidence reinforcing the theoretical argument I made in Chapter 3: whether or not a shift to a normal distribution on an attribute is indicative of depolarization or polarization is dependent on the shape and dispersion of the prior state of the distribution. In the case of gay rights, the aggregate shift to an on average ‘moderate’ position from the consensus position evident in 1974 is polarization. Aggregate moderation, in this instance, is evidence of increasing conflict (i.e. polarization). As we can see from the distributions in 2004 relative to that in 1974, opinion on gay rights has shifted from one extreme (consensus - unimodality) to the other (conflict - bimodality).

Polarization Trends in Gay Rights Attitudes, GSS 1973-2006

The consistent question-wording of the GSS homosexual relations item permit the direct comparison of polarization models on gay rights. The basic univariate statistics on gay rights from the GSS time-series are reported in Table 5.5. Note the sixth-tenths of a point increase in the mean as well as the three-tenths of a point increase in the standard deviation. Given the variable is scaled from zero to four; these are significant changes in the aggregate public opinion on homosexual relations towards greater polarization on gay rights over the time series.



While it is true the mean is moving towards the center on gay rights issues, as I noted previously, this reflects a move towards greater disagreement in the American public on gay rights. It is evidence of conflict, not, as some have concluded, moderation. Changes in means, as they relate to polarization, must be assessed in context: in relation to the shape of the distribution prior to the change and what the direction of the shift in the mean indicates about that change. Take the example of a move of the mean towards the center of the scale. Some culture war skeptics have misinterpreted the change in mean attitudes on social issues (attitudes that have become more centrally located on the NES scales) as evidence against polarization. We see this change in means with gay rights (i.e. trend of the mean moving towards the center of the attitudinal scale). But, in fact, this change is actually evidence of polarization. The prior distribution was consensual and unimodal. The shift of the mean towards the center, in this case, indicates a loss of that consensus, as the consensus position from the 1970’s and the 1980’s was located near the extreme of one side of the gay rights attitude scale. The change in the

Table 5.5: GSS – Public Opinion on Homosexual Relations 1973-2006

YEAR

MEAN

STAND DEV

KURTOSIS

ECD

SAMPLE N

1973

1.559

1.037

0.809

61.7

1,448

1974

1.620

1.099

0.246

57.5

1,412

1975

1.658

1.125

0.016

55.9

-

1976

1.696

1.151

-0.214

54.2

1,426

1977

1.654

1.127

0.040

57.0

1,453

1978

1.637

1.119

0.171

57.9

-

1980

1.620

1.111

0.302

58.7

1,397

1982

1.587

1.086

0.532

61.4

1,771

1983

1.607

1.100

0.364

60.2

-

1984

1.627

1.114

0.195

59.0

1,412

1985

1.590

1.097

0.449

61.6

1,484

1986

1.552

1.068

0.821

63.9

-

1987

1.514

1.039

1.193

66.3

1,750

1988

1.544

1.063

0.915

64.0

937

1989

1.633

1.139

0.144

58.5

980

1990

1.555

1.068

0.810

63.5

872

1991

1.609

1.134

0.332

59.5

926

1993

1.851

1.264

-0.965

44.3

1,012

1994

1.863

1.281

-1.015

43.2

1,884

1996

2.021

1.340

-1.434

32.2

1,784

1998

2.076

1.349

-1.534

28.6

1,753

2000

2.068

1.348

-1.533

30.0

1,697

2002

2.182

1.382

-1.710

22.0

884

2004

2.108

1.366

-1.606

26.8

868

2006

2.174

1.379

-1.696

22.3

1,908

standard deviation permits a more straightforward interpretation: increasing standard deviations connote increasing dispersion which connotes increasing political polarization on gay rights.

The ECD measure (extreme category difference: top category minus bottom category on the scale) is the difference between the percentage of respondents that chose the “always wrong” category and those that chose the “nothing wrong at all” with homosexual relations category. This is an important measure with respect to polarization because it provides a direct measure of the changes in the mass at the tails of the opinion distribution on gay rights. Recall, it isn’t enough to have extreme opinions on an issue represented in society in order to generate social conflict. Those opinions have to be held by a sufficient mass of the citizenry in order to provide a venue for political and partisan activism and conflict. If the two extremes have equal mass, then the ECD measure will be zero. The ECD measure shows that there was a large difference between the two theoretical extremes of the gay rights opinion in the 1970’s and into the 1980’s, reflecting the consensus against gay rights that existed at that time. The declining difference between the two extremes tracks the loss of mass at the “anti-gay rights” extreme and the increasing mass at the “pro-gay rights” extreme. This decline in the difference between the two extreme categories is evidence of an increase in dispersion and bimodality. This difference was at its maximum in 1988, right around the point where public opinion on gay rights began to shift. The low water mark (illustrating the equalization trend in the two categories, i.e. conflict) was the 22 percent difference in 2002. There is a quite marked decline in the consensus on gay rights, with a large number of citizens declining to rate homosexual relations as “always wrong” and a corresponding increase in the number of Americans who believe there is nothing wrong with homosexual relations. Also reported in Table 5.5 is the sample N for each year the GSS collected data on the homosexual relations questions. Recall that the statistics for the 1975, 1978, 1983, and 1986 years were interpolated (hence there is no sample N to report).

Table 5.6 reports the polarization regressions of the trends in the mean, standard deviation, coefficient of variation, kurtosis, and extreme category difference models. All indicate a trend towards greater dispersion, and hence increasing conflict on gay rights—increasing polarization in the attitudes of the American public on homosexuals.38 The best model, in terms of goodness-of-fit (.669), is the trend model on the standard deviation in gay rights attitudes. This demonstrates a significant increase in dispersion in opinion on gay rights for the mass public, a key aspect of polarization. The positive coefficient (0.01) indicates an average increase in the standard deviation (i.e. dispersion) of opinion on homosexual relations. The positive coefficient in the mean model (0.019) shows that the average

TABLE 5.6 Trends in Polarization of Attitudes on Homosexual Relations 1973 - 2006

Model:

gr = b0 + b1(year) + e

MEAN MODEL

S.D. MODEL

KURTOSIS MODEL

EXTREME CAT

DIF MODEL



P.E

(S.D.)



0.019***

(0.003)


0.010***

(0.001)


-0.072***

(0.014)


-1.202***

(0.193)


Intercept

(S.D.)



-35.017

(5.662)



-18.900

(2.941)


142.393

(27.384)


2439.294

(383.205)



R2

.647

.669

.542

.628

N

25

25

25

25

Pr < |t|

<.0001

<.0001

<.0001

<.0001

* significant at .10 level

** significant at .05 level

***significant at .01 level

response on gay rights in the GSS has moved away from the “always wrong” consensus of the 1970’s and early 1980’s to the contemporary divided opinion on gays today. Again, while this trend is towards the center of the distribution, it is evidence of increasing conflict on gay rights. The ‘anti’ side of the gay rights debate has lost supporters as a percentage of the total population (mass) and the ‘pro’ side of the gay rights debate has gained: drawing the mean on gay rights attitude towards the middle. Both models explain over sixty percent of the variation in the standard deviation and the mean respectively.

The negative coefficient (-0.072) in the Kurtosis model indicates that the distribution of attitudes on gay rights has become more bi-modal over the past three decades. As can be seen in Table 5.5, the distribution goes negative in 1993 and is negative in every survey year after to the present. At the beginning of the series, the kurtosis for the GSS gay rights attitude measure is at a 0.809. By the end of the series, it stands at -1.696. Recall that kurtosis is centered at zero for the normal distribution (mesokurtic). So the distribution on gay rights attitudes flips from leptokurtic to platykurtic over the course of the past three decades. The model shows there has been a significant increase in bimodality in the distribution of opinion on gay rights for the mass public, explaining over half of the variance in kurtosis over the time series relative to the mean kurtosis value (.542). The ECD model shows the declining consensus against gay rights over the time period, with the AW category losing, on average, 1.202 percent of its mass every year of the time-series relative to the NW category. Over half of the variance in the Kurtosis model and sixty-three percent of the variance in the ECD model is explained by the progress of time. Both suggest increasing bimodality on gay rights. Overall, Table 5.6 shows powerful evidence of increasing polarization on gay rights in the American public and is completely consistent with the culture wars thesis. There is increasing dispersion on gay rights (standard deviation model), the extreme categories have become more balanced as opinion has shifted away from extreme opposition to gay rights while extreme support has increased (ECD model), there is increasing bimodality as indicated by the negative coefficient from the kurtosis model, and the mean has shifted away from the anti-gay side of the opinion scale, as we would expect in a shift from consensus located near one pole of the scale towards conflict on the issue (mean model).

Analysis – Partisan Polarization Trends in Public Opinion on Gay Rights

While we have established the loss of consensus on most gay rights issues and on gay rights overall that has occurred over the last three decades, as I noted in Chapter 3, it isn’t mere issue polarization that makes for a culture war. In order for a polarized issue to become politically relevant, it has to be relevant to political competition. The political parties are a prime mover in American political competition. Parties define and shape political conflict, educate and motivate their supporters on issues, and seek to gain supporters by taking advantage of issues that divide the American public. An issue that is polarized and where the parties align along the cleavage of opinion on that issue is the essence of what makes for a cultural war. In the American political system, the threshold for an issue to gain salience among partisans and make its way into the debate over public policy is relatively large given the bias in the electoral system against minority factions (see Durverger’s Law). The GSS item on homosexual rights provides an opportunity to assess the trend in partisan opinion on gay rights and compare the trends in the differences both between the parties and within the parties.

Issues that divide the public and gain political salience present an opportunity for parties to gain supporters by appealing to one side or the other on the issue. Issues on which there is consensus, or which do not lend themselves to policy alternatives, or cross-cut well established partisan cleavages do not lend themselves to partisan and political competition (absent a realignment on the cross-cutting issue). Gay rights falls within the former classification. As a consensus issue in the 1970’s, neither of the major parties had it as a major plank in their party platforms, it was not an issue upon which their candidates campaigned, and consequently the mass public did not identify the parties with a position on gay rights nor did they align themselves with the parties, in whole or in part, based on their attitudes towards gay rights and their perceptions of the parties vis-à-vis gay rights.

That began to change midway through the 1980’s. As the consensus on gay rights died and the emerging social conflict on gay rights emerged, the prospect for partisan competition, partisan alignment on either side of the gay rights debate, and the institution of policy alternatives on gay rights became a possibility. If this story on gay rights is correct, then we would expect that the mass public would increasingly associate the parties with a position on gay rights, and that partisan identifiers would, overtime, increasingly identify with the party’s position on gay rights (either through conversion of partisan identifiers to the pro or anti positions on gay rights or the adoption of a party id by supporters and opponents of gay rights). Thus we would expect to see the party identifiers diverge on their attitudes on gay rights concomitant with the emerging cultural battle over homosexuals and their place in society. This is exactly what we see over this time period, as illustrated in Figures 5.7 - Figures 5.12. The broad classification of party identifiers reflects the emergent partisan polarization on gay rights and this polarization is evident for each of the party identification categories of respondents.



FIGURE 5.6: GSS – Homosexual Relations are Wrong 1973-2006, by Party Identification



FIGURE 5.7: GSS – Homosexual Relations are Wrong 1973-2006, Republican /Democrat Difference

Figure 5.6 illustrates the change in average opinion on homosexual relations among Republican, Democrat, and Independent identifiers in the time series.39 Note that in the beginning of the series in 1973, Republicans, Democrats and Independents are unified in their opinion on whether homosexual relations are wrong. Ninety percent of the respondents, irrespective of party identification, believed that there was something wrong with homosexual relations. The implications for a ‘culture war’ here are clear. In the 1970’s there wasn’t a “culture” war…certainly not on the hot button social issue of gay rights. There was an obvious consensus against homosexuals. No partisan advantage to be had by taking an opposing position and supporting gay rights (indeed, doing so at the time could have rated as political suicide). While some separation occurs over the course of the next decade, there was still relatively little difference (about 5%) between Republicans, Democrats, and Independents on the ‘wrongness’ of homosexual relations. In the early part of the 1990’s, just about the time that Hunter identified the existence of an emerging culture war, Republicans and Democrats begin to increasingly separate on gay rights. As can be seen in Figure 5.7, the difference between Republicans and Democrats tops ten percent and steadily increased through the end of the time series (maxing out at a 20% difference).

This trend in increasing partisan differences is apparent in each of the categories of partisanship on the 7-point scale. As one might expect, the largest disparity exists between strong Republican identifiers and strong Democratic identifiers. In 1973, there was no difference between strong Republicans and strong Democrats on homosexual relations (Figure 5.8). By 2006, forty percent of strong Democrats saw nothing wrong with homosexual relations compared with just fifteen percent of strong Republicans. While the differences are not as marked among weak identifiers (Figure 5.9) or independent leaners (Figure 5.10), there is still an apparent and distinct difference between Republicans and Democrats on gay rights completely consistent with the parties aligning in opposition on attitudes

FIGURE 5.8: GSS – Homosexual Relations are Not Wrong 1973-2006, Strong Republicans and Democrats



FIGURE 5.9: GSS – Homosexual Relations are Not Wrong 1973-2006, Weak Republicans and Democrats



FIGURE 5.10: GSS – Homosexual Relations are Not Wrong 1973-2006, Independent Leaners



FIGURE 5.11: GSS – Homosexual Relations are Not Wrong 1973-2006, Independents

towards homosexuals. The alignment isn’t perfect, however. All three of the parties evidence the overall trend towards increasing tolerance of homosexuals. However, note that Republicans who think there is something wrong with homosexual relations has only declined about 15 percent over the last three decades. The increase in support for gays among Democrats is twice that (Figure 5.6). The behavior of independents is interesting in the later part of the time-series. In the late 1990’s independents tracked closer to the Democrats in their tolerance of homosexuals, but by 2006 they had ticked back to a position just below that of the Republicans. While one can only speculate as to the cause, the prominence of gay marriage as the prime gay rights issue in this decade and the legal interventionism on gay marriage may have produced a backlash against gay rights among Independents.



Table 5.7: Party Means & Standard Deviation for Public Attitudes on Homosexual Relations 1971-2007

year

dem mean

dem s.d.

rep mean

rep s.d.

1973

1.584

1.053

1.512

1.004

1974

1.669

1.142

1.510

0.987

1975

1.688

1.153

1.579

1.049

1976

1.706

1.164

1.648

1.111

1977

1.658

1.142

1.608

1.064

1978

1.654

1.142

1.542

1.018

1980

1.650

1.141

1.475

0.972

1982

1.579

1.079

1.548

1.051

1983

1.629

1.115

1.538

1.038

1984

1.678

1.151

1.528

1.025

1985

1.593

1.100

1.553

1.067

1986

1.572

1.086

1.497

1.013

1987

1.550

1.072

1.440

0.958

1988

1.625

1.129

1.473

0.991

1989

1.774

1.216

1.473

1.014

1990

1.685

1.155

1.440

0.973

1991

1.737

1.231

1.495

1.043

1993

2.054

1.345

1.642

1.136

1994

2.009

1.341

1.698

1.184

1996

2.152

1.383

1.818

1.262

1998

2.257

1.298

1.792

1.223

2000

2.189

1.371

1.807

1.231

2002

2.385

1.410

1.880

1.294

2004

2.401

1.422

1.843

1.249

2006

2.419

1.415

1.919

1.293

Finally, I examine the evidence on public attitudes on gay rights as it relates to the partisan differences on the polarization measures. Table 5.7 reports the means and standard deviations for the GSS homosexual rights item for the time series. Again, the trend away from the consensus against homosexual relations is apparent for both parties, but more marked among Democrats who register a half-point closer to the ”nothing wrong at all” category than Republicans. I estimate six different partisan and party difference models on the trend in attitudes towards gay rights (Table 5.8). Intercepts, parameter estimates, and goodness-of-fit statistics are reported for each model. All of the models are statistically significant at the .10 level and all but one of the models is significant at the .01 level. I estimate trend models for the mean response on and the standard deviation of the GSS homosexual relations item for both Republicans and Democrats. I further estimate a linear model on the trend in the difference between the average Republican and Democrat mean response and standard deviation for the time series. The models for the means and standard deviations for both Republicans and Democrats illustrate the loss of consensus and move towards conflict on gay rights issues. The positive coefficients on the mean trend indicate that both Republicans and Democrats have moved away from the previous consensus against homosexuals. The standard deviation models indicate that for both Republicans and Democrats, there is greater diversity on gay rights today than there was three decades ago.

However, note the distinct difference in the goodness-of-fit measure. The R2 for Democrats on the mean trend model indicates that twenty percent more of the variance in the mean on gay rights is explained by the passage of time in comparison to the Republican mean model. The model explains seventy-five percent of the variance for the Democrats but only fifty percent for Republicans. There is a similar distinction between the Republican and Democrat S.D. models, with the Democratic model explaining more of the variance than the Republican model. The answer, I believe, is found in the difference models. The positive coefficient in the R-D mean difference model (0.015) shows that there is a significant increasing difference between Republicans and Democrats on gay rights (R2 = .786). While



Table 5.8: Partisan Trends in Public Attitudes on Homosexual Relations 1971-2007

Model:

gr = b0 + b1(year) + e

Trend:

Conflict or

Consensus


Intercept

(S.E.)


Parameter Estimate

(S.E.)


R2

N

Mean Democrats

on Gay Relations



CONF

-50.167

(6.420)



0.026

(0.003)


***

.741

25

Standard Deviation

Dems on G. R.




CONF


-20.101

(2.759)


0.011

(0.001)


***

.723


25


Mean Republicans

on Gay Relations




CONF

-20.332

(4.403)



0.011

(0.002)



***

.519

25

Standard Deviation

Reps on G. R.




CONF

-16.338

(2.962)


0.008

(0.001)



***

.601

25

REP – DEM Mean Difference on G.R.


CONF

-29.835

(3.271)


0.015

(0.002)


***

.786

25

REP – DEM S.D.

Difference on G.R.




CONF

-3.763

(2.075)


0.002

(0.001)


*

0.132

25

* significant at .10 level

** significant at .05 level

***significant at .01 level

the difference between S.D. is significant, with Democrats experiencing greater intra-party dispersion than the Republicans, it is a relatively small difference (.002) and a concomitant poorly fit model (R2 = .132).

In the 1970’s and the early years of the 1980’s a clear consensus against homosexuals and strong opposition to gay rights existed. As a consequence, the parties largely ignored the issue. The mass public did not differentiate the parties on gay rights, partisan elites had no incentive to stake out positions on gay rights or propose policy alternatives related to the issue, and gay rights was well off the public agenda for all but the small enclaves of gay rights activists. This consensus died sometime in the middle of the 1980’s. Support for gay rights emerged and the same time opposition to gay rights declined. What emerged was an issue ripe for political and partisan conflict. The growing mass of citizens in favor of gay rights encouraged the Democratic Party to woo those citizens (either to convert them to the party or as a consequence of the increasing number of Democratic party identifiers who supported gay rights) by offering policy alternatives such as the protection of homosexuals in the workplace and permitting gays to openly serve in the military. The parties became to be identified with specific positions on gay rights (Republicans opposed; Democrats in support) and gay rights supporters and opponents became an identifiable cleavage between the parties (and thus distinct groups that would need to be satisfied by policies in order to generate money and electoral support). This trend is apparent and significant in all of the partisan measures on gay rights opinion over the past three decades. The parties have clearly diverged on gay rights. In three short decades the Republicans and Democrats in the mass electorate went from having nearly identical viewpoints on gay rights to distinct

Figure 5.12: Which Party is Closest to Respondent on Gay Marriage, 200540

and divergent views on this social issue. It is thus no surprise that gay rights are a significant player in the partisan battles in the culture war from the 1990’s to the present. The American public is divided on gay rights, and hence are divided in their support for either party on gay rights issues (Figure 5.12).



Conclusion

I believe the evidence presented in this chapter paints a very clear picture on political polarization and the culture wars. In the early 1970’s and through the middle part of the 1980’s there existed a strong consensus in the American public that was barely tolerable of and expressly hostile towards homosexuals. In the later part of the 1980’s public attitudes began to undergo a radical transformation in attitudes on gay rights. An increasing number of Americans began to see “nothing wrong” with homosexual relations and were increasingly tolerant of and comfortable with gays. The result of this formation of a second and oppositional camp on gay rights was part and parcel of the emerging culture war in the early part of the 1990’s. Today, whether it be the spectacle of George Takei (Mr. Sulu) getting married to his long-time partner in San Francisco or the heated legal battles over Defense of Marriage acts versus state court-imposed gay marriage, the battle lines are clearly drawn. There is social conflict on gay rights. Here I have shown that a host of gay rights issues have moved from consensus to conflict (with but a few countervailing examples of gay rights attitudes moving from conflict to consensus). Using two different datasets of public attitudes on gay rights, I have shown that there is an overall trend from consensus to conflict on attitudes towards homosexuals, homosexuality, and homosexual rights.

Furthermore, I have shown that the parties have reacted to and aligned along this cultural divide. As public opinion on gay rights diversified, it created an opportunity for the parties to use gay rights to compete for votes. As a consequence the Democrats have aligned much more strongly with the pro-gay rights faction with Republicans sticking with the anti-gay rights faction. The parties have recognized, reacted to, and mobilized on the cultural battle over gay rights. And there is little evidence that this war will end any time in the near future.

This chapter has presented a clear case of increasing political polarization and conflict on one of the hot-button social issues of the day and one of the major fronts in the culture wars. It further serves to demonstrate the application of specific, empirical measures of polarization to test whether opinion on an issue has become more dispersed, more bimodal, and more extreme (e.g. more polarized). However, this chapter on gay rights, while assessing polarization in an important social issue and a key aspect of the culture wars, is narrow in scope. There are other social issues relevant to partisan and political competition and conflict. Furthermore, there are non-social issue dimensions such as economic policy, government spending and regulation, and foreign policy in which polarization (or depolarization) can occur. In the next chapter I expand upon the analysis presented here by employing the measures of polarization to look not just at the range of other social issues that have sparked the culture war, but also at the other major dimensions of political conflict in the United States.




And it’s not surprising then [that] they get bitter, they cling to guns or religion or antipathy to people who aren’t like them or anti-immigrant sentiment or anti-trade sentiment as a way to explain their frustrations.” – Barak Obama, April 2008

Download 4.38 Mb.

Share with your friends:
1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8   9   10   ...   42




The database is protected by copyright ©ininet.org 2024
send message

    Main page