Boundary Work In Psychology: The Uneasy Relationship Between Scientific and “Pop” Psychology Barbara Lusk, Department of Psychology Introduction



Download 82.06 Kb.
Date31.03.2018
Size82.06 Kb.
#44888
Boundary Work In Psychology: The Uneasy Relationship Between Scientific and “Pop” Psychology

Barbara Lusk, Department of Psychology


Introduction
No scientific discipline has captured the public’s imagination or seeped into its consciousness as much as psychology. After all, psychology deals with the essence of being human – how we think, how we feel, and how we behave. Even more seductive, psychology claims to have the tools to uncover the reasons (both obvious and hidden) for our own thoughts, emotions, and actions, as well as those of others. At the same time, perhaps no discipline has been as misunderstood. Little wonder. Dr. Phil, now entering his tenth year on television, draws between four and five million viewers each week. At its peak, The Dr. Laura Program was the second-highest-rated radio show after The Rush Limbaugh Show, and was heard on more than 450 radio stations. “Dear Abby” routinely receives 10,000 letters and emails each week, appears in close to 1,400 newspapers, and has a daily readership of more than 110 million worldwide. Roughly 3,500 self-help books are published annually, with only a handful written or screened by scientific experts, and the total yearly “take” of this type of book is estimated to be roughly twelve billion dollars. So-called experts boasting psychology credentials appear regularly on cable news programs offering their opinions of this or that headline of the day. Psychologists are portrayed in various guises in such television programs as HBO’s In Treatment and re-runs of Fraser. New wave psychology is lampooned in Showtime’s Enlightened. Portrayals of psychologists and the discipline in film are no less problematic. Given this, it is no surprise that students entering my psychology classes have a rather distorted perception of what practitioners do and what the discipline has to offer.
As one might expect, the academic discipline of psychology, concerned that these popular manifestations of the field damage its scientific credibility and threaten its standing, has pushed back. In fact, an uneasy, ambivalent, and emotionally charged relationship between scientific psychology and “public” or “pop” psychology has dogged the discipline since its inception. Long before psychology became an established and legitimate discipline, there was a public psychology, rooted in such pseudo-sciences as phrenology, physiognomy, graphology, mesmerism, spiritualism, and mental healing. These were featured in books, newspaper columns, home study courses, psychology clubs, mail-order degrees, and in the scores of popular psychology magazines that began appearing in the 1890s. More to the point, much of the public found this work to be both useful and comforting. The new academic psychologists, struggling to forge their professional identity and attain both legitimacy and respectability, found themselves in a quandary – how to demarcate scientific psychological inquiry from the works associated with quacks and hucksters, while at the same time selling the practical value of their discipline and earning public support.
For this study grant I propose to investigate how academic psychologists address this ongoing quandary. In so doing, I shall trace the history of the uneasy relationship between academic and “pop” psychology, beginning in the late 1800s and continuing today. As shall be seen, academic psychologists have been engaged – and continue to be engaged – in what historians and sociologists of science call “boundary work”: the conscious drawing of borders and boundaries that demarcate the division between areas of science and non-science. As expected, these boundaries, once created, become somewhat fluid as they are typically challenged and then defended by the faithful. And the stakes are high.
Rationale
During the course of my research in the history of psychology I have heard many scholars make reference to the discipline’s “identity crisis” – and with good reason. My own background in the field is a case in point. Educated and trained as a clinical psychologist, my perspective was skewed. I was taught that psychology is, foremost, a helping profession, its experimental branch serving to support its applied function. This limiting view, I have learned during my years of teaching, has been a consistent misperception among students enrolled in my credit classes, as well as the individuals in the community who attend my public presentations. Confounding things even further is the wide gulf between what the lay public thinks psychology is – which is ordinarily some variant of “pop psychology” – and the actual state of affairs.
This proposed research, then, in addition to furthering my own intellectual growth as a historian of psychology through examination of the continual relationship between professional and “pop” psychology, will also be of significant value for my students. The psychology major is one of the most popular options at the undergraduate level across the country. In fact, the major is typically among the top three choices nationally. In the case of students at the community college level who have not yet decided upon a major, psychology classes also rank as some of the most popular courses offered. General psychology in particular meets one of the core requirements for many degree and certificate plans. When examining the motivation of students who take a psychology class, beyond the “it’s required” rationale, many are initially drawn to the course out of a desire to understand themselves better or “to help people.” Their surprise when confronted with the complexity of the discipline and with the discovery that psychology is defined as the scientific study of mind and behavior is a matter of some interest. I believe that a careful study of the very foundation of these misperceptions can only serve to enhance the education of these students who should be exposed to much more than the one to two page overview of history that is in most introductory psychology texts. With this grant, I shall be able to fill this void by providing a more nuanced and complete account of psychology’s history and the somewhat shaky interaction between scientific and “pop” psychology.

Additionally, members of the community who enroll in non-credit psychology classes can also benefit from a more accurate understanding of the discipline. For the past six years, I have been an invited speaker for Collin College’s Seniors Active in Learning Visiting Professor series. Here I present a four-week seminar on topics related to the social sciences. These older and presumably wiser men and women frequently express many of the same mistaken notions typical of the “credit” students and often sign up for my classes for similar reasons. Thus, I plan to suggest a series of lectures devoted to the history of psychology as detailed in this study grant proposal.


Last, after completion of this research I intend to present papers at the annual meetings of the American Psychological Association, the discipline’s national organization, and Cheiron, the International Society of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. Eventually, I will submit papers on this topic to peer-reviewed journals in the history of psychology.
Study Plan
I am requesting funding for the Summer II, 2013 semester. As such, I have organized my readings into eight sections. The sources I shall be examining are written, for the most part, by eminent scholars in the history of psychology. Additional sources, typically accounts and reviews of what has come to be known as “pop” psychology that appear in the more general mass media, are included as well. In all, these sources consist of four types: (1) extensive biographies of prominent figures in the early development of psychology – such as Hugo Munsterberg, John Watson, and Henry H. Goddard – when its applied aspects were used as “selling points” for the discipline, (2) specific historical accounts of the development of both academic and popular psychology that appear in the major journals in the behavioral sciences, (3) feature articles that appear in publications aimed at a more general audience such as Psychology Today, (3) “new wave” magazines, and (4) movie and television reviews of films and programs depicting psychologists and/or the profession.
In what follows, I present a general overview of the topics I shall be covering in each of the eight sections I have created for the study. Included in this narrative is a small sampling of the sources to be examined. For a complete listing of topics and sources, please consult the section-by-section bibliography provided below.
I shall begin this study by reading historical accounts of some of the popular precursors to academic psychology. The sources listed in Section 1 focus upon the dissemination of mesmerism in America in the mid-nineteenth century, animal magnetism in southern New England, the growth and popularity of phrenology, and the public’s fascination with spiritualism and psychic phenomenon.
The efforts of academic psychologists to establish clear boundaries between their work and those on the fringe of the discipline are the main focus of the sources I shall read in Section 2. Two specific cases of boundary work take center stage. The first case, psychic phenomena, illustrates that academic psychologists can differ among themselves when engaged in building boundaries. As shall be seen, one of psychology’s founding fathers, G. Stanley Hall, with the assistance of Amy Tanner, expended considerable effort to debunk psychic phenomena. At the same time, however, another giant in the field, William James, had a strong interest in spiritualism and became, along with other luminaries such as Richard Hodgson, a founder and member of the American Society for Psychical Research, a group whose primary goal was to prove the existence of ghosts, spirits, and psychic phenomena as determined by “rigorous scientific investigation.” Michael Pettit’s account of Hall and Tanner’s indefatigable efforts to expose one of Boston’s most re-known psychic mediums, Leonora Piper, and Deborah Blum’s chronicle of James’ efforts in Ghost Hunters: William James and the Search for Scientific Proof of Life after Death provide interesting accounts of these two opposing positions. Analyzed together, this controversy embodies the conflict that is the focus of this study grant.
The second case to be examined is the discipline’s response to the growing popularity of Freudian analysis as it seeped into the public’s consciousness. Although most psychologists ignored psychoanalysis when it first arrived in the United States, by 1920 many took note of its rising popularity and began to argue strenuously that the radical subjectivity at the core of the Freudian approach threatened the core scientific assumptions of the discipline. I will read both Morawski’s and Hornstein’s excellent accounts of the reception of Freud in the United States and the various maneuvers used by mainstream psychologists in their attempts to ward off this new threat to their discipline. To provide some needed comparisons, I shall also consult Rapp’s work on the public’s reception of Freud in Great Britain.
Next, in Section 3 I turn to mainstream psychology’s efforts to craft its image in such a way that the public could realize the applied value of the academic discipline. To set the stage, I shall begin with Dennis’ examination of the press coverage psychology received in the New York Times. I will then look more specifically at how academic psychologists touted the tools of their trade in their scholarly publications. Psychology, it was claimed, could make meaningful contributions in a number of institutional settings.
Hugo Munsterberg was one of the foremost advocates and popularizers of the new applied psychology. Hale’s acclaimed biography of Munsterberg will undoubtedly provide examples of his application of psychological principles in the criminal justice system – he created the field of “forensic psychology” – and business settings – his work on organizational efficiency launched the field of “industrial psychology.” Benjamin’s work will provide other examples, such as the use of psychology in advertising.
Next, I will focus on the development and application of intelligence testing in the United States. Zenderland’s excellent biography of Henry H. Goddard, one of the leading eugenicists in the United States, Fancher’s collective biography of many of the stalwarts in this field, and Sokal’s edited volume on psychological testing and American society will trace this history during the hey-day of eugenics in the United States. Of particular interest will be psychologists’ arguments that they could provide indispensable aid to the war effort by administering these tests to weed out defectives from serving on the frontlines, while also identifying those recruits best suited for leadership roles.
No discussion of the applied aspects of psychology in the 1920s is complete without a review of its contributions to the arduous task of childrearing. Leading psychologists such as Mary Cover Jones and John B. Watson published books and manuals instructing parents as to the best methods of childrearing, thereby creating individuals with a strong work ethic and high morals. Rutherford’s biographical chapter of the former and Buckley’s biography of the latter will outline these efforts to introduce behavioral techniques into the family, as will Doroshow’s interesting account of the popular use of classical conditioning – in the form of an alarm sounding – to deal with the problem of bedwetting.
To close this section, I shall investigate two instances of the applied role of psychology in sports. In the first, Fuchs recounts the testing of Babe Ruth’s swing and reaction time in a psychological laboratory at Columbia University; in the second Graef and his colleagues discuss how one of the pioneers in professional football, Paul Brown, introduced psychological testing into the clubhouse.
Although early academic psychologists were able to push the study of psychic phenomena outside of the boundaries of the discipline, it was of course unable to stem public interest in research of this type. As a result, by the 1940s the new field of parapsychology was emerging as a legitimate scientific specialty, with its own sources of private funding, professional organizations and journals, and in fact, some positions within established psychology departments. Readings in Section 4 will focus upon the development of this fringe field, how it has been embraced in the “pop” psychology movement, and the rather heated response of mainstream psychology as it attempted to fortify its boundaries.
Next, in Section 5, I shall focus on the “radical behaviorism” of B.F. Skinner, one of the most publicly identifiable figures in psychology of the last century, and progenitor of what came to be known as the “behavior modification” movement. Relying upon the work of Rutherford, I will trace how his work was portrayed, interpreted, misinterpreted, and received by the general public throughout his career. This case, too, will illustrate boundary work in psychology, as behavior modification became a staple in the new-wave psychology and self-help movements that are the subjects of the next two sections.
Psychology was not immune to the forces at play in the 1960s. More to the point, this era was considered to be the golden age of “new wave” psychology. Primal scream therapy, nude psychotherapy, age regression therapy, Erhard Seminar Training (EST), and encounter groups were becoming acceptable and exciting means of exploring the self in the public’s view. Psychotherapy was no longer reserved for the mentally ill; rather, being in therapy was a means for exploring a higher form of consciousness. Once again, mainstream psychology believed that interlopers were storming the gates of respectable scientific psychology. Readings in Section 6 will focus on these new wave “pop psych” innovations, as well as the discipline’s response to what were perceived as incursions into conventional therapeutic practices.
Much of the general public’s ideas about psychology are derived from the mass media, and for years academic psychologists have noted and sought to correct what they viewed as the distorted depiction of psychologists and the discipline. Movies such as Bringing Up Baby in which Katherine Hepburn is imprisoned by a Freudian quack; What’s New, Pussycat, where Peter O’Toole details his sex life to Dr. Fritz Fassbender, played by Peter Sellers; Ken Kesey’s One Flew Over the Cuckoo’s Nest with its use of electroconvulsive therapy; Mel Brook’s comedic portrayal of Dr. Richard Thorndyke, the administer of the Psycho-Neurotic Institute for the Very, Very Nervous in High Anxiety; or Robin Williams’ unconventional therapeutic approach in Good Will Hunting, they argue, do more harm than good. The films either belittle the field, present caricatures of practitioners and their methods, or give viewers totally unrealistic expectations about the therapeutic process, should they seek professional assistance.
Psychologists and their work on television suffer from the same maladies, and then some. Here, they are not only portrayed as comedic fictional characters, such as in The Bob Newhart Show, and Fraser, but also as serious therapists, as in the HBO series In Treatment. At the same time, commentators with degrees in psychology on various cable news networks give their diagnoses – often in sound-bytes – of people in the news, while still others, like Dr. Phil or Dr. Ruth, dispense their expert advice to all and sundry characters willing to go on television. Readings in Section 7, then, will focus on the characterization of psychologists and the discipline in film and television and the consequences of such portrayals.
Last, Section 8 concludes with an examination of the twelve billion dollar self-help industry, evident when one visits any free-standing bookstore and discovers that the self-help section occupies a significant amount of physical space, or when one ventures into the virtual world of self-help and finds thousands of websites. Self-help, of course, has a long lineage and academic psychologists have been engaged in skirmishes with this genre ever since Dale Carnegie’s How to Win Friends and Influence People appeared in 1936. Other classics in this genre include Norman Vincent Peale’s The Power of Positive Thinking, published in 1952, and, more recently, M. Scott Peck’s The Road Less Traveled, Wayne Dyer’s Your Erroneous Zones, Leo Buscaglia’s Living, Loving, and Learning, Thomas Harris’ I’m OK – You’re OK, John Gray’s Men Are From Mars, Woman Are From Venus, and, of course, the entire Chicken Soup for the Soul series. Readings in this section will analyze the history of this type of work, outline its many guises, discuss factors that appear to enhance its appeal to the public, and illustrate, yet again, the tactics and strategies used by academic psychologists responding to what they perceive to be another assault on their discipline.

Summer Session Study Plan

Boundary Work In Psychology: The Uneasy Relationship Between Scientific Psychology And Popular Psychology
I. Popular precursors to Academic Psychology: Mesmerism, Phrenology, Spiritualism, and Psychic Phenomena
Schmit, David, (2005). “Re-visioning American Antebellum Psychology: The Dissemination of Mesmerism, 1836 – 1854, History of Psychology, 8, 403 – 434.
Quinn, Sheila O., (2007). “How Southern New England Became Magnetic North: The Acceptance of Animal Magnetism, History of Psychology, 10, 231- 248.
Cooter, Roger. (1984). The Cultural Meaning of Popular Science: Phrenology and the Organization of Consent in Nineteenth-Century Britain, Cambridge University Press.
Pickren, Wade and Rutherford, Alexandra. (2010). “Forging a Psychological Sensibility: from Religion to Psychical Research.” Pp. 75 – 80 in A History of Modern Psychology in Context, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey.

II. Boundary Work: From Séance and Pseudoscience to Science

Pickren, Wade and Rutherford, Alexandra. (2010). “Boundary Work and the New Psychology: Establishing the Center and Marking the Periphery.” Pp. 80 – 89 in A History of Modern Psychology in Context, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey.

Blum, Deborah. (2006). Ghost Hunters: William James and the Search for Scientific Proof of Life after Death. Penguin Press, New York, New York.

Pettit, Michael. (2008). “The New Woman as Tied-Up Dog: Amy E. Tanner’s Situated Knowledges,” History of Psychology, Vol. 11, No. 3, 145 – 163.

Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. (2010). “The Public’s Psychology: A History of the Popular Psychology Magazines in America.”

Morawski, Jill G. and Hornstein, Gail A., (1991). “Quandary of the Quacks: The Struggle for Expert Knowledge in American Psychology, 1890-1940.” Pp. 106 – 33 in Brown, JoAnne and van Keuren, David K. (Eds), The Estate of Social Knowledge. The Johns Hopkins symposia in comparative history, Vol. 19, Baltimore, MD, US: Johns Hopkins University Press.



  1. The Diffusion of Freud into the Public Domain

Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. and Dixon, David N. (1996). “Dream Analysis by Mail.” American Psychologist, Vol. 51, No, 5, 461 – 468.
Hornstein, Gail A. (1992). “The Return of the Repressed: Psychology’s Problematic Relations with Psychoanalysis, 1909 – 1960,” American Psychologist, 47, 254 – 263.
Rapp, Dean, “The Early Discovery of Freud by the British General Educated Public, 1912–1919, Social History of Medicine 3 (2): 217 – 243.
Rapp, Dean, (1988). “The Reception of Freud by the British Press: General Interest and Literary Magazines, 1920–1925,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 24(2): 191-201.
III. Selling The New “Applied Psychology” to an Eager Public

Adams, Grace. (1928). “The Decline of Psychology,” The American Mercury, December 1928, pp. 450-453.


Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. (1986). “Why Don’t They Understand Us?: A History of Psychology’s Public Image,” American Psychologist, Vol. 41, No. 9, 941 – 946.
Dennis, Paul M. (2011). “Press Coverage of the New Psychology by the New York Times During the Progressive Era,” History of Psychology, Vol. 14, No. 2, 113 – 136.
Dennis, Paul M. (2002). “Psychology’s Public Image in ‘“Topics of the Times”’: Commentary from the Editorial Page of the New York Times between 1904 and 1947, Journal of History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 38 (4), 371 – 392.


  1. Business/organizational efficiency

Hale, Matthew, (1980). Human Science and Social Order: Hugo Munsterberg and the Origins of Applied Psychology. Temple University Press.

Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. (2003) “Harry Hollingworth and the Shame of Applied Psychology.” Pp. 38 – 56 in Baker, David B. (ed), Thick Description and Fine Texture, University of Akron Press, Akron, Ohio.

Benjamin, L.T., & Baker, D.B. (2004). Industrial-organizational Psychology: The New Psychology and the Business of Advertising. Pp. 118 – 121 in From Séance to Science: A History of the Profession of Psychology in America, California: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.



  1. Intelligence testing: Eugenics and the War Effort

Fancher, Raymond E. (1985). The Intelligence Men—Makers of the IQ Controversy, W. W. Norton and Company, New York and London.


Pickren, Wade and Rutherford, Alexandra. (2010) “Psychologists as Testers: Applying Psychology, Ordering Society.” Pp. 118 – 147 in A History of Modern Psychology in Context, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey.

Sokal, Michael M. (1987). Psychological Testing and American Society,



1890 – 1930, Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick and London.
Zenderland, Leila. (1998). Measuring Minds—Henry Herbert Goddard and the Origins of American Intelligence Testing, Cambridge University Press.


  1. Child rearing

Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr., and Nielsen-Gammon, Elizabeth. (1999). “B.F. Skinner and Psychotechnology: The Case of the Heir Conditioner,” Review of General Psychology, Vol. 3, 155 – 167.


Buckley, K. W. (1989). Mechanical Man: John Broadus Watson and the Beginnings of Behaviorism. New York: The Guilford Press.
Doroshow, Deborah Blythe. (2010). “An Alarming Solution – Bedwetting, Medicine, and Behavioral Conditioning in Mid-Twentieth-Century America,”

Isis, 101:312 – 337.
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2006). “Mother of Behavior Therapy and Beyond: Mary Cover Jones and the Study of the ‘Whole Child’.” In D. A. Dewsbury, L. T. Benjamin, Jr., and M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. VI, chapter 12). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association.


  1. Sports

Fuchs, Alfred H. (2009). “Psychology and Baseball: The Testing of Babe Ruth. Pp. 144 – 167 in Green, Christopher and Benjamin, Ludy T., Psychology Gets in the Game: Sport, Mind, and Behavior, 1880 – 1960, University of Nebraska Press.


Graef, Stephen T., Kornspan, Alan S. and Baker, David (2009). “Paul Brown: Bringing Psychological Testing to Football. Pp. 230 – 252 in Green, Christopher and Benjamin, Ludy T., Psychology Gets in the Game: Sport, Mind, and Behavior, 1880 – 1960, University of Nebraska Press.
IV. Skirmishes on the Border: Parapsychology, ESP and Popular Culture Revisited
Allison, Paul D. (1979). “Experimental Parapsychology as a Rejected Science. Pp. 271 – 291 in Wallis, Roy, (ed.), On the Margins of Science: The Social Construction of Rejected Knowledge, University of Keele.
Collins, H.M. and Pinch, Trevor (1979). “The Construction of the Paranormal: Nothing Unscientific Is Happening.” Pp. 237 – 270 in-Wallis (ed.) On the Margins of Science: The Social Construction of Rejected Knowledge, University of Keele.
Hess, David J. (1992). “Disciplining Heterodoxy, Circumventing Discipline: Parapsychology, Anthropologically.” Pp. 191 – 222 in Hess, David and Layne, Linda (Eds.), Knowledge and Society, Vol. 9: The Anthropology of Science and Technology, JAI Press.
V. Behavior Modification: B.F. Skinner and the Diffusion of his work into Popular Culture
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2000). “Radical Behaviorism and Psychology’s Public: B.F. Skinner in the Popular Press, 1934 -1990,” History of Psychology, Vol. 3, No. 4, 371 – 395.
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2003). “B.F. Skinner’s Technology of Behavior in American Life: From Consumer Culture to Counterculture,” Journal of History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 39 (1), 1 – 23.
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2009). Beyond the Box – B.F. Skinner’s Technology of Behavior from Laboratory to Life, 1950s – 1970s, University of Toronto Press.
VI. New-Wave Psychology’s Public Allure
Williams, Paul and Edgar, Brian. (2008). “Up Against the Wall: Primal Therapy and ‘the Sixties,’” European Journal of American Studies, Special Issue, May 2008.
Nicholson, Ian. (2007). “Baring the Soul: Paul Bindrim, Abraham Maslow and Nude Psychotherapy,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 43 (4), 337 – 359.
Tavris, Carol. (2003). “Mind Games: Psychological Warfare Between Therapists and Scientists,” The Chronicle of Higher Education, Vol. 49, Issue 25: B7ff.
VII. Psychology’s Portrayal on the Big and Little Screens


  1. Psychology in the Cinema

Gabbard, Glen O. and Gabbard, Krin, (2005). Psychiatry and the Cinema, 2nd ed., American Psychiatric Press, Inc.


McDonald, Andrew and Walter, Garry, (2009). “Hollywood and ECT,” International Review of Psychiatry, 21(3): 200–206.
Orchowski, Lindsay M., Spickard, Brad A, and McNamara, John R. (2006). “Cinema and the Valuing of Psychotherapy: Implications for Clinical Practice.” Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, Vol. 37, No. 5, 506 – 514.
Sandis, Constantine (2009). “Hitchcock’s Conscious Use of Freud’s Unconscious,” Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 3/2009, pp. 56 – 81.
Schultz, Harriet T. (2005). “Good, Bad, Ugly: Movie Therapy and Movies’ Impact on Therapy: Symposium General Summary.” American Psychological Association Symposium Proceedings, Division 46, American Psychological Association, Inc.
Sleek, Scott. (1998). “How are Psychologists Portrayed on Screen?” APA Monitor, Vol. 29, No. 11.


  1. Psychology on Television

Becker, Anne. (2008). “Rise of the TV Shrink: Why New Shows on Psychotherapy Sessions Keep Viewers on Edge of Couch,” Broadcasting and Cable.


Fischoff, Stuart. (1995). “Confession of a TV Talk Show Shrink,” Psychology Today, September 1995.
Vogel, David L., Gentile, Douglas A., and Kaplan, Scott A. (2008). “The Influence of Television on the willingness to Seek Therapy,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 64 (3), 276- 295.
Klonoff, Elizabeth A., (1983), “A Star Is Born: Psychologists and the Media,” Professional Psychology: Research and Practice 16 (6): 847-854.
VIII. The Twelve Billion-dollar Self-help Industry

Valiunas, Algis (2010). “The Science of Self-Help,” The New Atlantis, Sumer, 2010, pp. 85 – 100.


Zimmerman, T. S., Holm, K. E., and Haddock, S. A. (2001). “A Decade of Advice for Women and Men in the Best-Selling Self-Help Literature,” Family Relations, Vol. 50, No. 2, pp. 122-133.
Rosen, Gerald M., Glasgow, Russell E., Moore, Timothy E. (2003). “Self-help Therapy: The Science and Business of Giving Psychology Away.” Pp. 399 – 424 in Lilienfeld, Scott O., Lynn, Steven Jay, and Lohr, Jeffrey M. (Eds), Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology, New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Ehrenreich, Barbara (2009). Bright-sided: How the Relentless Promotion of Positive Thinking Has Undermined America. Macmillan Press.
Boundary Work In Psychology: The Uneasy Relationship Between Scientific Psychology And Popular Psychology
Complete Bibliography
Adams, Grace. (1928). “The Decline of Psychology,” The American Mercury, December 1928, pp. 450-453.
Allison, Paul D. (1979). “Experimental Parapsychology as a Rejected Science. Pp. 271 – 291 in Wallis, Roy, (ed.), On the Margins of Science: The Social Construction of Rejected Knowledge, University of Keele.
Becker, Anne. (2008). “Rise of the TV Shrink: Why New Shows on Psychotherapy Sessions Keep Viewers on Edge of Couch,” Broadcasting and Cable.

Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. (2003) “Harry Hollingworth and the Shame of Applied Psychology.” Pp. 38 – 56 in Baker, David B. (ed), Thick Description and Fine Texture, University of Akron Press, Akron, Ohio.


Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. (1986). “Why Don’t They Understand Us?: A History of Psychology’s Public Image,” American Psychologist, Vol. 41, No. 9, 941 – 946.
Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. (2010). “The Public’s Psychology: A History of the Popular Psychology Magazines in America.”
Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. and Dixon, David N. (1996). “Dream Analysis by Mail.” American Psychologist, Vol. 51, No, 5, 461 – 468.
Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. and Nielsen-Gammon, Elizabeth. (1999). “B.F. Skinner and Psychotechnology: The Case of the Heir Conditioner,” Review of General Psychology, Vol. 3, 155 – 167.

Benjamin, Ludy T., Jr. and Baker, David B. (2004). Industrial-organizational Psychology: The New Psychology and the Business of Advertising. Pp. 118 – 121 in From Séance to Science: A History of the Profession of Psychology in America, California: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.


Blum, Deborah. (2006). Ghost Hunters: William James and the Search for Scientific Proof of Life after Death. Penguin Press, New York, New York.
Buckley, K. W. (1989). Mechanical Man: John Broadus Watson and the Beginnings of Behaviorism. New York: The Guilford Press.
Collins, H.M. and Pinch, Trevor (1979). “The Construction of the Paranormal: Nothing Unscientific Is Happening.” Pp. 237 – 270 in-Wallis (ed.) On the Margins of Science: The Social Construction of Rejected Knowledge, University of Keele.
Cooter, Roger. (1984). The Cultural Meaning of Popular Science: Phrenology and the Organization of Consent in Nineteenth-Century Britain, Cambridge University Press.
Dennis, Paul M. (2002). “Psychology’s Public Image in ‘“Topics of the Times”’: Commentary from the Editorial Page of the New York Times between 1904 and 1947, Journal of History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 38 (4), 371 – 392.
Dennis, Paul M. (2011). “Press Coverage of the New Psychology by the New York Times During the Progressive Era,” History of Psychology, Vol. 14, No. 2, 113 – 136.
Doroshow, Deborah Blythe. (2010). “An Alarming Solution – Bedwetting, Medicine, and Behavioral Conditioning in Mid-Twentieth-Century America,” Isis, 101:312 – 337.
Ehrenreich, Barbara (2009). Bright-sided: How the Relentless Promotion of Positive Thinking Has Undermined America. Macmillan Press.
Fancher, Raymond E. (1985). The Intelligence Men—Makers of the IQ Controversy, W. W. Norton and Company, New York and London.
Fischoff, Stuart. (1995). “Confession of a TV Talk Show Shrink,” Psychology Today, September 1995.
Fuchs, Alfred H. (2009). “Psychology and Baseball: The Testing of Babe Ruth. Pp. 144 – 167 in Green, Christopher and Benjamin, Ludy T., Psychology Gets in the Game: Sport, Mind, and Behavior, 1880 – 1960, University of Nebraska Press.
Gabbard, Glen O. and Gabbard, Krin, (2005). Psychiatry and the Cinema, 2nd ed., American Psychiatric Press, Inc.
Graef, Stephen T., Kornspan, Alan S. and Baker, David (2009). “Paul Brown: Bringing Psychological Testing to Football. Pp. 230 – 252 in Green, Christopher and Benjamin, Ludy T., Psychology Gets in the Game: Sport, Mind, and Behavior, 1880 – 1960, University of Nebraska Press.
Hale, Matthew, (1980). Human Science and Social Order: Hugo Munsterberg and the Origins of Applied Psychology. Temple University Press.

Hess, David J. (1992). “Disciplining Heterodoxy, Circumventing Discipline: Parapsychology, Anthropologically.” Pp. 191 – 222 in Hess, David and Layne, Linda (Eds.), Knowledge and Society, Vol. 9: The Anthropology of Science and Technology, JAI Press.


Hornstein, Gail A. (1992). “The Return of the Repressed: Psychology’s Problematic Relations with Psychoanalysis, 1909 – 1960,” American Psychologist, 47, 254 – 263.
Klonoff, Elizabeth A., (1983), “A Star Is Born: Psychologists and the Media,” Professional Psychology: Research and Practice 16 (6): 847-854.
McDonald, Andrew and Walter, Garry, (2009). “Hollywood and ECT,” International Review of Psychiatry, 21(3): 200–206.
Morawski, Jill G. and Hornstein, Gail A., (1991). “Quandary of the Quacks: The Struggle for Expert Knowledge in American Psychology, 1890-1940.” Pp. 106 – 33 in Brown, JoAnne and van Keuren, David K. (Eds), The Estate of Social Knowledge. The Johns Hopkins symposia in comparative history, Vol. 19, Baltimore, MD, US: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Nicholson, Ian. (2007). “Baring the Soul: Paul Bindrim, Abraham Maslow and Nude Psychotherapy,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 43 (4), 337 – 359.


Orchowski, Lindsay M., Spickard, Brad A, and McNamara, John R. (2006). “Cinema and the Valuing of Psychotherapy: Implications for Clinical Practice.” Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, Vol. 37, No. 5, 506 – 514.
Pettit, Michael. (2008). “The New Woman as Tied-Up Dog: Amy E. Tanner’s Situated Knowledges,” History of Psychology, Vol. 11, No. 3, 145 – 163.

Pickren, Wade and Rutherford, Alexandra. (2010). “Forging a Psychological Sensibility: from Religion to Psychical Research.” Pp. 75 – 80 in A History of Modern Psychology in Context, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey.

Pickren, Wade and Rutherford, Alexandra. (2010). “Boundary Work and the New Psychology: Establishing the Center and Marking the Periphery.” Pp. 80 – 89 in A History of Modern Psychology in Context, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey.

Pickren, Wade and Rutherford, Alexandra. (2010) “Psychologists as Testers: Applying Psychology, Ordering Society.” Pp. 118 – 147 in A History of Modern Psychology in Context, John Wiley and Sons, Hoboken, New Jersey.

Quinn, Sheila O. (2007). “How Southern New England Became Magnetic North: The Acceptance of Animal Magnetism, History of Psychology, 10, 231- 248.
Rapp, Dean. (1988). “The Early Discovery of Freud by the British General Educated Public, 1912–1919, Social History of Medicine 3 (2): 217 – 243.
Rapp, Dean. (1988). “The Reception of Freud By the British Press: General Interest and Literary Magazines, 1920–1925,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 24(2): 191-201.
Rosen, Gerald M., Glasgow, Russell E., and Moore, Timothy E. (2003). “Self-help Therapy: The Science and Business of Giving Psychology Away.” Pp. 399 – 424 in Lilienfeld, Scott O., Lynn, Steven Jay, and Lohr, Jeffrey M. (Eds), Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology, New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2000). “Radical Behaviorism and Psychology’s Public: B.F. Skinner in the Popular Press, 1934 -1990,” History of Psychology, Vol. 3, No. 4, 371 – 395.
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2003). “B.F. Skinner’s Technology of Behavior in American Life: From Consumer Culture to Counterculture,” Journal of History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 39 (1), 1 – 23.
Rutherford, Alexandra. (2006). “Mother of Behavior Therapy and Beyond: Mary Cover

Jones and the Study of the ‘Whole Child’.” In D. A. Dewsbury, L. T. Benjamin, Jr., and M. Wertheimer, (Eds.), Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology (Vol. VI, chapter 12). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association.


Rutherford, Alexandra. (2009). Beyond the Box – B.F. Skinner’s Technology of Behavior from Laboratory to Life, 1950s – 1970s, University of Toronto Press.
Sandis, Constantine (2009). “Hitchcock’s Conscious Use of Freud’s Unconscious,” Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 3/2009, pp. 56 – 81.
Schmit, David. (2005). “Re-visioning American Antebellum Psychology: The Dissemination of Mesmerism, 1836 – 1854, History of Psychology, 8, 403 – 434.
Schultz, Harriet T. (2005). “Good, Bad, Ugly: Movie Therapy and Movies’ Impact on Therapy: Symposium General Summary.” American Psychological Association Symposium Proceedings, Division 46, American Psychological Association, Inc.
Sleek, Scott. (1998). “How are Psychologists Portrayed on Screen?” APA Monitor, Vol. 29, No. 11.
Sokal, Michael M. (1987). Psychological Testing and American Society, 1890 – 1930, Rutgers University Press, New Brunswick and London.
Tavris, Carol. (2003). “Mind Games: Psychological Warfare Between Therapists and Scientists,” The Chronicle of Higher Education, Vol. 49, Issue 25: B7ff.
Valiunas, Algis (2010). “The Science of Self-Help,” The New Atlantis, Sumer, 2010, pp. 85 – 100.
Vogel, David L., Gentile, Douglas A., and Kaplan, Scott A. (2008). “The Influence of Television on the Willingness to Seek Therapy,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 64 (3), 276- 295.
Williams, Paul and Edgar, Brian. (2008). “Up Against the Wall: Primal Therapy and ‘the Sixties,’” European Journal of American Studies, Special Issue, May 2008.
Zenderland, Leila. (1998). Measuring Minds—Henry Herbert Goddard and the Origins of American Intelligence Testing, Cambridge University Press.
Zimmerman, T. S., Holm, K. E., and Haddock, S. A. (2001). “A Decade of Advice for Women and Men in the Best-Selling Self-Help Literature,” Family Relations, Vol. 50, No. 2, pp. 122-133.
Download 82.06 Kb.

Share with your friends:




The database is protected by copyright ©ininet.org 2024
send message

    Main page