PRINTED Amrein, A.L., Berliner, D. C. (2003). The effects of high-stakes testing on student motivation and learning. Educational Leadership, 60 (5), 32-38.
-accessible through ERIC
PRINTED Berliner, D.C. (1993). The 100-year journey of Educational Psychology: From interest, to disdain, to respect for practice. In T.K. Fagan and G.R. Vandenbos (Eds.) Exploring Applied Psychology Origins and Critical Analysis: Master Lectures in Psychology. Washington, D.C: APA.
– accessible on course website
PRINTED Boekaerts, M. (2002). Motivation to Learn. Geneva, Switzerland: International Bureau of Education Publications Unit.
-accessible through ERIC
NOT ON ERIC Brown, A.L. (1994). The advancement of learning. Educational Researcher, 23 (8), 4-12.
-accessible through ERIC
Chance, P. (1992). The rewards of learning. Phi Delta Kappan, 74 (3), 200-207.
-accessible on course website
Chance, P. (1993). Sticking up for rewards. Phi Delta Kappan, 74 (10), 787-790.
-accessible on course website, included with Kohn (1993) article
NOT ON ERIC Cohen, D.K. (1998). Dewey’s Problem. The Elementary School Journal, 98 (5), 427-446.
-selection: read pages 427-437, until “Society and School”.
-accessible through ERIC
PRINTED Cohn, A. (2001). Positive Behavioral Supports: Information for Educators. National Association of School Psychologists. Retrieved March 28, 2009 from: http://www.nasponline.org/resources/factsheets/pbs_fs.aspx
-accessible on course website
PRINTED Heffner, C.L. (2001). Psychology 101. AllPsych Online. Retrieved March 28, 2009 from: http://allpsych.com/psychology101/index.html
-selection: Chapter 4: Learning Theory and Behavioral Psychology (read all three sections of this chapter)
-accessible on course website
Kamin, L. (1995). The pioneers of IQ testing. In R. Jacoby and N. Glauberman (Eds.) The Bell Curve Debate: History, Documents, Opinions. New York: Times Books
– accessible on course website
Kohn, A. (1993). Rewards verses learning: A response to Paul Chance. Phi Delta Kappan, 74 (10), 783-787.
-accessible on course website
PRINTED Lave, J. (1985). Introduction: Situationally specific practice. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 16 (3), 171-176. -accessible on course website PRINTED McLeod, S.A. (2007). Simply Psychology. Retrieved March 28, 2008 from: http://www.simplypsychology.pwp.blueyonder.co.uk/ -selection 1: “Piaget's Theory of Child Development” – accessible on course website -selection 2: “Vygotsky's Theory of Social Development” – accessible on course website
PRINTED Moran, S., Kornhaber, M., & Gardner, H. (2006). Orchestrating multiple intelligences. Educational Leadership, 64 (1), 22-27.
-accessible through ERIC
PRINTED Pugh,K. & Girod, M. (2007). Science, Art, and Experience: Constructing a Science Pedagogy from Dewey’s Aesthetics. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 18, 9-27.
-accessible through ERIC
ALREAD READ Skinner’s utopia: Panacea, or path to hell? (1971, September 20). Time, 47-53.
- accessible on course website
Smith, M.K. (2009). Communities of practice. The Encyclopedia of Informal Education. Retrieved June 20, 2010 from http://www.infed.org/biblio/communities_of_practice.htm.
-accessible on course website
NOT ON ERIC Tomlinson, S. (1997). Edward Lee Thorndike and John Dewey on the science of education. Oxford Review of Education, 23 (3), 365-383.
-accessible through ERIC
Vygotsky, L. (1978). In Mind in Society. (Trans. M. Cole). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
– selection: Chapter 4, Interaction between Learning and Development (pp. 79-91)
- accessible on course website
Widmayer, S. (N.D.) Schema Theory: An Introduction. Retrieved March 28, 2009 from: http://www2.yk.psu.edu/~jlg18/506/SchemaTheory.pdf
-accessible on course website
Willingham, D.T. (2008). What is developmentally appropriate practice? American Educator, 32 (2), 34-39
- accessible on course website
http://www.nasponline.org/resources/factsheets/pbs_fs.aspx
Positive Behavioral Supports Information for Educators By Andrea M. Cohn What is Positive Behavioral Support?
Positive Behavioral Support (PBS) is an empirically validated, function-based approach to eliminate challenging behaviors and replace them with prosocial skills. Use of PBS decreases the need for more intrusive or aversive interventions (i.e., punishment or suspension) and can lead to both systemic as well as individualized change.
PBS can target an individual student or an entire school, as it does not focus exclusively on the student, but also includes changing environmental variables such as the physical setting, task demands, curriculum, instructional pace and individualized reinforcement. Thus it is successful with a wide range of students, in a wide range of contexts, with a wide range of behaviors.
Blending behavioral science, empirically validated procedures, durable systems change and an emphasis on socially important outcomes, PBS always involves data-based decision making using functional behavioral assessment and ongoing monitoring of intervention impact.
According to IDEA '97, PBS is the recommended form of intervention for dealing with challenging behavior in children with disabilities. In Fiscal Year 1999, the U.S. Department of Education's Office of Special Education Programs in collaboration with Safe and Drug Free Schools supported a Center for Positive Behavioral Interventions and Supports with a grant of almost $600,000. Information from this center is available at www.pbis.org. Additionally, the U.S. government continues to support a project at the University of Kansas (Beach Center on Families and Disability) to promote programs related to the personal needs of infants, toddlers, children and youth with disabilities; this program supports the use of PBS to help children with disabilities who demonstrate challenging behavior.
Why Do We Need PBS?
Problem behavior is the single most common reason why students are removed from regular classrooms. Even though students with extreme problem behavior represent only 20% of school enrollment, they can account for more than 50% of behavioral incidents.
Harsh punishment and zero tolerance policies have not been effective at either improving behavioral climate in schools, or preventing students with problem behaviors from entering the juvenile justice system.
Three years after being excluded from school, almost 70% of these youth have been arrested.
Failure to implement IDEA, due to a lack of incentives or negative attitudes toward children with challenging behaviors by administrators, policy makers and school personnel, is unacceptable. Students should not be excluded from school based solely upon inappropriate social behavior. Appropriate services can readily address and modify many of these behaviors, leading to more positive outcomes than simple punishment.
How is PBS Implemented in School Settings?
PBS is based on behavioral theory; problem behavior continues to occur because it is consistently followed by the child getting something positive or escaping something negative. By focusing on the contexts and outcomes of the behavior, it is possible to determine the functions of the behavior, make the problem behavior less effective and efficient, and make the desired behavior more functional. This often involves changing systems, altering environments and teaching new skills, as well as focusing on the problem behavior.
The most crucial part of devising PBS plans is the Functional Behavioral Assessment (FBA), which reveals information about the antecedents, consequences, and frequency of challenging behavior. FBAs also help to identify any co-occurring variables. Conducting FBAs doubles the success rate of an intervention.
PBS plans are individualized and data-based and include procedures for monitoring, evaluating and reassessing the process. PBS should be a collaborative effort among parents, school psychologists, teachers, counselors and administrators; all partners should be committed to the plan and its implementation. PBS is more effective when it includes the target individual as well as other significant individuals (i.e., peers, teachers, and parents).
High fidelity of implementation is required to maximize outcomes; therefore, interventions should be applied by educators in the school environment. School psychologists are ideally qualified to conduct FBAs, implement PBS plans and train other educators and parents in behavioral intervention techniques.
What Are the Benefits of PBS?
All students, both disabled and non-disabled, can benefit from PBS:
Research conducted over the past 15 years has shown that PBS is effective in promoting positive behavior in students and schools. Use of PBS as a strategy to maintain appropriate social behavior will make schools safer. Safer schools are more effective learning environments.
Schools that implement system-wide interventions also report increased time engaged in academic activities and improved academic performance.
Schools that employ system-wide interventions for problem behavior prevention indicate reductions in office discipline referrals of 20-60%.
Appropriately implemented PBS can lead to dramatic improvements that have long-term effects on the lifestyle, functional communication skills, and problem behavior in individuals with disabilities.
A review of research on PBS effectiveness showed that there was over a 90% reduction in problem behavior in over half of the studies; the problem behavior stopped completely in over 26% of the studies.
How Can We Improve Implementation of PBS?
Although it is commendable that many states require functional behavior assessments before the development of significant behavioral interventions, they often occur reactively, or after the behavior has become a significant problem (i.e., after a student's behavior results in multiple suspensions or a drug/weapons infraction). After a crisis occurs, the focus is on punishment and exclusion. Additionally, school-based interventions commonly consist of unproven strategies and are implemented by staff who lack the training to deal with the problems effectively. When coordination is lacking among schools and other agencies, the primary responsibility for behavior is placed on families, who receive little support. Effective implementation of PBS includes:
An FBA, conducted when the problem behavior is first observed or as a proactive activity
Focus both on prevention of problem behaviors and early access to effective behavior support.
Culturally competent, family-friendly behavior support
Implementation with sufficient intensity and precision to produce behavioral gains that have a significant and durable impact on the academic, social and living options available to the student.
References
Carr, E. G., Horner, R. H., Turnbull, A. P., Marquis, J. G., McLaughlin, D. M., McAtee, M. L., Smith, C. E., Ryan, K. A., Ruef, M. B., Doolabh, A., & Braddock, D. (1999). Positive behavior support for people with developmental disabilities: A research synthesis. Washington, D.C.: American Association on Mental Retardation.
Heumann, J., & Warlick, K. (2001). Prevention research & the IDEA discipline provisions: A guide for school administrators. Available: www.ed.gov/offices/OSERS/OSEP/adminbeh.web.pdf.
Horner, R. H., Crone, D. A., & Stiller, B. (2001, March). The role of school psychologists in establishing positive behavior support: Collaborating in systems change at the school-wide level. Communiqué, 29(6), 10-12.
Skiba, R. J. (2000, August). Zero tolerance, zero evidence: An analysis of school disciplinary practice. (Policy Research Rep. No. SRS2).
Sugai, G., & Horner, R. (2001, June). School climate and discipline: Going to scale. The National Summit on the Shared Implementation of IDEA, Washington, D.C. Available at: www.ideainfo.org
U.S. Department of Education. (2000). Applying positive behavioral support in schools: Twenty-second Annual Report to Congress on the Implementation of the Individuals with Disability Act. Washington, D.C.: Author.
Walker, H. Colvin, G., & Ramsey, E. (1995). Antisocial behavior in public school: Strategies and best practices. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Andrea M. Cohn is a doctoral student in the school psychology program at the University of Maryland; this fact sheet was developed during her summer (2001) internship at NASP Headquarters.
©2001, National Association of School Psychologists—4340 East West Highway, #402, Bethesda, MD 20814
Are there any "ancient" ideas on the workings of learning that you feel still hold true today? Berliner credits Thorndike with advancing Educational Psychology while critiquing him for limiting it: what did Thorndike do right, according to Berliner, and what did he do wrong? How did Thorndike's views differ from those of the other founders of the field, James, Hall, and Dewey? Which of these theorists' ideas hold the most appeal for you, and why? Based on your experience, do you agree with Berliner that Educational Psychology has at times been greatly disconnected from schools? Do you see this changing, as Berliner does, or do you believe that the theory/practice divide is alive and well? How have you used Educational Psychology (or not!) in your own practice? How do you anticipate using it in the future?
What does the behaviorist account of learning look like? Which aspects of human learning, behavior, and activity do behaviorists study, and which aspects do they ignore? Why do behaviorists adopt this narrow view of human activity? How do classical and operant conditioning differ from one another? Can you think of some examples of both types of conditioning in classrooms? Does your classroom employ the reinforcements and/or punishments of operant conditioning? The associations of classical conditioning? What moral or philosophical issues might arise in the application of behaviorism to schooling? Where do you stand on the use of behaviorist theory and practice in schools?
Behaviorist learning is to change something on the outside to cause an action inside. Change something in the world around us to change something within us is the way I think about it. For example, if I was not fed for two days, and a pizza was put in front of me, I would try a variety of actions (e.g. sing, dance, sit, stand, etc) to get the food. When one (dancing) worked, I would do it again and again to get more pizza. Eventually, when I saw a pizza box or a pizza delivery person, I would automatically dance without thinking. This is classical conditioning. We develop responses to a stimulus that are not normal.
I can relate to classical learning. When I smell a certain kind of cologne, I automatically think back to an old boyfriend who I liked greatly and smile. It just happens involuntarily. In the classroom, I play some smooth jazz music during writing time to calm the students and turn the lights off. This automatically quiets the class and helps them focus on a task.
Operant conditioning is our actions to continue [or not continue] are determined by how someone reacts to our behavior. The behavior comes first and then the reaction. I used this method in my classroom last year. When students walked down the hallway quietly, got a compliment from a teacher, or were seated and ready within 2 ½ minutes, then I would put marbles in a jar. Full jar=an ice cream party.
I think a lot of “educational” computer/internet games fall into this category. I see this in my classroom on a website called MathWhizz which is used greatly in my school district. Students get taught a lesson and then do ten problems. If they get them all correct, they get points to buy a virtual pet and accessories. If not, the lesson gets recycled through at another time.
We, as educators, use behavioral learning techniques all the time. “Praise the good and ignore the bad.” The bad actions will cease when they see they see others get praised for positive behaviors. I do it all the time. [works great for K-2] As parents, I think it comes with the child in the delivery room and as educators, when we get our first teaching license for TSPC.
Behaviorist learning does not take into consideration the health of the body or mind, emotion, upbringing, and environmental issues (e.g. religion, language, culture, socioeconomic levels, parental involvement, etc.) It takes out the individuality and makes us machines. It dehumanizes us.
Piagetian theory and Schema theory are both aspects of Cognitive theory. In general, how does the learner (the student) look different from a Cognitive perspective than from a Behaviorist perspective? Looking at Piaget in particular, how have his theories changed our thinking about child development? According to Willingham, how has developmental theory been updated since Piaget's time? How can developmental theory be put to use in the classroom, and how should it not be used? According to Widmayer, how does Schema theory describe learning, and what instructional strategies can be derived from Schema theory? In general, how do the methods and outcomes of these Cognitive theories differ from the methods and outcomes associated with Behaviorism? How can a teacher use the methods associated with each type of theory to achieve different sorts of classroom outcomes?
Piaget brought a new approach to psychology world. The Behaviorists focused on changing the environment to change the thought patterns inside. No value was given to thought processes or developmental changes. Piaget determined that humans go through stages of cognitive development through his life, some never completing levels. As babies, they learn how to think through movement, senses, and trial and error. (Sensorimotor). Then move to preoperational where the idea is that everyone sees, hears, and feels the same way as they do, and they cannot see from other’s view. Language comes into play. Concrete operational takes place around 7-11 years old where thinking is done logically about concrete objects, and finally formal operational is where abstract thinking, inferring, predicting, and other high-level thinking occurs.
Wellingham refutes Piaget’s theory of schema and the four stages. He states that the development depends on the task, the child, and the moment in time. A child can perform a task two different ways on two different days. The proof depends what you ask to them to understand and how you want them to show how they understand it. If a child is not being able to show proficiency, it may be they did not understand the concept or they could not show you their knowledge in your desired way. He suggested not instructing on developmental levels but constantly change strategies and methods of presenting problems and solving problems.
Widmayer’s focus is schema. Schema is like the “building blocks” or the organization in your head and its rules. It is organized by meaning and experience to help a person predict and understand. She encourages educators to activate prior knowledge, use preview strategies when reading text, and make connections from current schema to the new schema being introduced.
What is the relationship between learning and development in Vygotsky's theories? How does language acquisition demonstrate this relationship? How is this relationship different from that put forth by Skinner (Behaviorist) or Piaget (Cognitive)? What else is different about Vyogotsky's ideas, and his picture of human nature, compared to the Behaviorist and Cognitive perspectives? What is the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD), and how can this concept guide developmental research and educational practice? What are some classroom practices associated with Vygotskian theory, and how are they different from Behaviorist and Cognitive practices in terms of form and of expected results? How does Lave describe the difference between her own Situative perspective and the Cognitive ideas that influence schooling? How might the adoption of Lave's perspective influence classroom practice?
Vygotsky theory of Social Development Theory is quite different than Piaget’s theory of Stage Theory of Cognitive Development. Piaget identified four stages of cognitive development where development precedes learning; on the other hand, Vgotysky’s theory states that social learning precedes development. Humans learn from social interaction/culture and are born with the foundation to for intellectual development; whereas, Piaget states that motor reflexes and sensory abilities start out the cognition journey.
Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) is an idea that is used as frequently as pencils in the classroom. The Zone of Proximal Development is the area where students cannot learn without the help of a peer/teacher/adult. It is basically the area between what a student knows (can do independently) and what is not known (what a child can learn/do with assistance). As educators, we try to always teach in the ZPD. For example, if a child can read independently at a fourth grade level, the teacher will teach that child using a fifth grade text or even a sixth grade text with the use of scaffolding.
Scaffolding is taking high-level text/concepts and breaking it down, so children can understand it. The idea is to “teach to the high and bring up the low.” Many teachers will group students heterogeneously, so advanced students can help the lower students make meaning of a concept. (ZPD) This is called collaborative learning. Scaffolding and collaborative learning relate to Vygotsky’s theory of making meaning through social interaction. I use both of these techniques when using the GLAD (Guided Language Acquisition Model). I teach content while drawing pictures, use actions while singing songs, and meet with a person from each team and teach them something, and they go back and teach their teams. These strategies are quite effective.
Brown has a story to tell about the development of educational theory. How does this story compare with the narrative that has been developed through our readings in ED 611? How does Brown treat Behaviorist, Cognitive, and Situative theories, and which theories does she think should be employed in schools? What are some of her specific recommendations involving classroom methods and the ideas of learning and development that should be in schools? How do you see yourself employing these practices or ideas? According to Smith, what are "communities of practice" and how does they change how we think about learning and about teaching? How do Smith's recommendations, based on various Situative theorists, compare to Brown's? How could structuring your classroom as a "community of practice" (or, perhaps, as multiple and varied communities) change what teaching, learning, and achievement look like? How could Smith's and Brown's recommendations increase the effectiveness of your teaching?
http://allpsych.com/psychology101/learning.html
Chapter 4: Learning Theory and Behavioral Psychology
Introduction to Learning Theory and Behavioral Psychology
Learning can be defined as the process leading to relatively permanent behavioral change or potential behavioral change. In other words, as we learn, we alter the way we perceive our environment, the way we interpret the incoming stimuli, and therefore the way we interact, or behave. John B. Watson (1878-1958) was the first to study how the process of learning affects our behavior, and he formed the school of thought known as Behaviorism. The central idea behind behaviorism is that only observable behaviors are worthy of research since other abstraction such as a person’s mood or thoughts are too subjective. This belief was dominant in psychological research in the United Stated for a good 50 years.
Perhaps the most well known Behaviorist is B. F. Skinner (1904-1990). Skinner followed much of Watson’s research and findings, but believed that internal states could influence behavior just as external stimuli. He is considered to be a Radical Behaviorist because of this belief, although nowadays it is believed that both internal and external stimuli influence our behavior.
Behavioral Psychology is basically interested in how our behavior results from the stimuli both in the environment and within ourselves. They study, often in minute detail, the behaviors we exhibit while controlling for as many other variables as possible. Often a grueling process, but results have helped us learn a great deal about our behaviors, the effect our environment has on us, how we learn new behaviors, and what motivates us to change or remain the same.
Classical and Operant Conditioning
Classical Conditioning. One important type of learning, Classical Conditioning, was actually discovered accidentally by Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936). Pavlov was a Russian physiologist who discovered this phenomenon while doing research on digestion. His research was aimed at better understanding the digestive patterns in dogs.
During his experiments, he would put meat powder in the mouths of dogs who had tubes inserted into various organs to measure bodily responses. What he discovered was that the dogs began to salivate before the meat powder was presented to them. Then, the dogs began to salivate as soon as the person feeding them would enter the room. He soon began to gain interest in this phenomenon and abandoned his digestion research in favor of his now famous Classical Conditioning study.
Basically, the findings support the idea that we develop responses to certain stimuli that are not naturally occurring. When we touch a hot stove, our reflex pulls our hand back. It does this instinctually, no learning involved. It is merely a survival instinct. But why now do some people, after getting burned, pull their hands back even when the stove is not turned on? Pavlov discovered that we make associations which cause us to generalize our response to one stimuli onto a neutral stimuli it is paired with. In other words, hot burner = ouch, stove = burner, therefore, stove = ouch.
Pavlov began pairing a bell sound with the meat powder and found that even when the meat powder was not presented, the dog would eventually begin to salivate after hearing the bell. Since the meat powder naturally results in salivation, these two variables are called the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and the unconditioned response (UCR), respectively. The bell and salivation are not naturally occurring; the dog was conditioned to respond to the bell. Therefore, the bell is considered the conditioned stimulus (CS), and the salivation to the bell, the conditioned response (CR).
Many of our behaviors today are shaped by the pairing of stimuli. Have you ever noticed that certain stimuli, such as the smell of a cologne or perfume, a certain song, a specific day of the year, results in fairly intense emotions? It's not that the smell or the song are the cause of the emotion, but rather what that smell or song has been paired with...perhaps an ex-boyfriend or ex-girlfriend, the death of a loved one, or maybe the day you met you current husband or wife. We make these associations all the time and often don’t realize the power that these connections or pairings have on us. But, in fact, we have been classically conditioned.
Operant Conditioning. Another type of learning, very similar to that discussed above, is called Operant Conditioning. The term "Operant" refers to how an organism operates on the environment, and hence, operant conditioning comes from how we respond to what is presented to us in our environment. It can be thought of as learning due to the natural consequences of our actions.
Let's explain that a little further. The classic study of Operant Conditioning involved a cat who was placed in a box with only one way out; a specific area of the box had to be pressed in order for the door to open. The cat initially tries to get out of the box because freedom is reinforcing. In its attempt to escape, the area of the box is triggered and the door opens. The cat is now free. Once placed in the box again, the cat will naturally try to remember what it did to escape the previous time and will once again find the area to press. The more the cat is placed back in the box, the quicker it will press that area for its freedom. It has learned, through natural consequences, how to gain the reinforcing freedom.
We learn this way every day in our lives. Imagine the last time you made a mistake; you most likely remember that mistake and do things differently when the situation comes up again. In that sense, you’ve learned to act differently based on the natural consequences of your previous actions. The same holds true for positive actions. If something you did results in a positive outcome, you are likely to do that same activity again.
Classical and Operant Conditioning
Classical Conditioning. One important type of learning, Classical Conditioning, was actually discovered accidentally by Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936). Pavlov was a Russian physiologist who discovered this phenomenon while doing research on digestion. His research was aimed at better understanding the digestive patterns in dogs.
During his experiments, he would put meat powder in the mouths of dogs who had tubes inserted into various organs to measure bodily responses. What he discovered was that the dogs began to salivate before the meat powder was presented to them. Then, the dogs began to salivate as soon as the person feeding them would enter the room. He soon began to gain interest in this phenomenon and abandoned his digestion research in favor of his now famous Classical Conditioning study.
Basically, the findings support the idea that we develop responses to certain stimuli that are not naturally occurring. When we touch a hot stove, our reflex pulls our hand back. It does this instinctually, no learning involved. It is merely a survival instinct. But why now do some people, after getting burned, pull their hands back even when the stove is not turned on? Pavlov discovered that we make associations which cause us to generalize our response to one stimuli onto a neutral stimuli it is paired with. In other words, hot burner = ouch, stove = burner, therefore, stove = ouch.
Pavlov began pairing a bell sound with the meat powder and found that even when the meat powder was not presented, the dog would eventually begin to salivate after hearing the bell. Since the meat powder naturally results in salivation, these two variables are called the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and the unconditioned response (UCR), respectively. The bell and salivation are not naturally occurring; the dog was conditioned to respond to the bell. Therefore, the bell is considered the conditioned stimulus (CS), and the salivation to the bell, the conditioned response (CR).
Many of our behaviors today are shaped by the pairing of stimuli. Have you ever noticed that certain stimuli, such as the smell of a cologne or perfume, a certain song, a specific day of the year, results in fairly intense emotions? It's not that the smell or the song are the cause of the emotion, but rather what that smell or song has been paired with...perhaps an ex-boyfriend or ex-girlfriend, the death of a loved one, or maybe the day you met you current husband or wife. We make these associations all the time and often don’t realize the power that these connections or pairings have on us. But, in fact, we have been classically conditioned.
Operant Conditioning. Another type of learning, very similar to that discussed above, is called Operant Conditioning. The term "Operant" refers to how an organism operates on the environment, and hence, operant conditioning comes from how we respond to what is presented to us in our environment. It can be thought of as learning due to the natural consequences of our actions.
Let's explain that a little further. The classic study of Operant Conditioning involved a cat who was placed in a box with only one way out; a specific area of the box had to be pressed in order for the door to open. The cat initially tries to get out of the box because freedom is reinforcing. In its attempt to escape, the area of the box is triggered and the door opens. The cat is now free. Once placed in the box again, the cat will naturally try to remember what it did to escape the previous time and will once again find the area to press. The more the cat is placed back in the box, the quicker it will press that area for its freedom. It has learned, through natural consequences, how to gain the reinforcing freedom.
We learn this way every day in our lives. Imagine the last time you made a mistake; you most likely remember that mistake and do things differently when the situation comes up again. In that sense, you’ve learned to act differently based on the natural consequences of your previous actions. The same holds true for positive actions. If something you did results in a positive outcome, you are likely to do that same activity again.
Share with your friends: |