LINGUISTICS 105, WINTER 2016 ASSIGNMENT 8
Name: Section: Score: / 3 =
I. (6 points) Negative forms in some African languages. Below are some sets of verbs in three African languages that pair affirmatives and negatives of several verb forms. Fula is a West Atlantic language spoken by mainly nomadic cattle herders across West Africa. Tamazhaq is a Berber language spoken in Niger. Bade is a Chadic language spoken in northeastern Nigeria. Data here are from the Western dialect. 1
Fula: Doubled vowels = long vowels; y = IPA [j]. The prefixed ’o is the third singular subject agreement morpheme.
|
Active Voice
‘he washes (it)’
|
Middle Voice
‘he bathes’
|
Passive Voice
‘he/it gets washed’
|
General Past
|
’o-lootii
|
’o-lootake
|
’o-lootaama
|
Emphatic Past
|
’o-lootu
|
’o-looti
|
’o-loota
|
Negative Past*
|
’o-lootaayi
|
’o-lootaaki
|
’o-lootaaka
|
General Future
|
’o-lootay
|
’o-loototo
|
’o-lootete
|
Negative Future
|
’o-lootataa
|
’o-lootataako
|
’o-lootataake
|
*The Negative Past is the negative counterpart of both General Past and Emphatic Past.
Tamazhaq: ə = IPA [ɨ], ă = IPA [ʌ]. Singular subject agreement affixes are given to help with identifying other morphemes, but you will not have to account for them.
|
Perfective
‘X went out’
|
Neg. Perfective
‘X didn’t go out’
|
Imperfective
‘X be going out’
|
Neg. Imperfective
‘X be not going out’
|
1 sg.
|
əgmăda
|
wurəgmeda
|
əgammăda
|
wurəgəmmăda
|
2 sg.
|
təgmăda
|
wurtəgmeda
|
təgammăda
|
wurtəgəmmăda
|
3 m.sg.
|
igmăd
|
wurəgmed
|
igammăd
|
wurəgəmmăd
|
3 f.sg.
|
təgmăd
|
wurtəgmed
|
təgammăd
|
wurtəgəmmăd
|
Bade: All forms are as they would appear with third person subjects. Doubled vowels = long vowels; grave accent (à) = low tone, no accent = high tone, acute accent (á) = downstepped high. you do not need to consider tone for this problem.
|
Perfective
‘he verbed
|
Neg. Perfective
‘he didn’t verb’
|
Imperfective
‘he is verbing’
|
Neg. Imperfective
‘he is not verbing’
|
‘drink’
|
so
|
sàasam
|
à-sa
|
à-sam
|
‘catch’
|
gàfo
|
gàfàafam
|
à-gə̀fi
|
à-gə̀fím
|
‘hide’
|
də̀psu
|
də̀psàasam
|
à-dəpsà
|
à-dəpsám
|
‘turn’
|
mə̀skə̀tu
|
mə̀skə̀tàatam
|
à-məskətà
|
à-məskətám
|
For each language identify the morphemes requested below. do not give full lexical entries. Just list the morphemes and any allomorphs, using our structuralist notation /x ~ y/ or /x ∞ y/.
Fula
Verb root:
Separate negative morpheme, if any:
Verb morphology associated with negation:
Tamazhaq: Remember that Tamazhaq has a root-and-pattern system. Use the Semiticist system to describe verb morphology.
Verb root:
Separate negative morpheme, if any:
Verb morphology associated with negation:
Bade
Verb roots:
Separate negative morpheme, if any:
Verb morphology associated with negation:
II. (6 points total) Korean negatives and dependent verb forms
(a) (1.5 points) Negative. Korean has several ways to express negation. “Short” form negatives place a negative marker before the verb, which is inflected for tense and politeness level. There are two negative markers: an and mos. Ho-Min Sohn (The Korean Language, CUP, 1999:389) describes the difference as follows:
In general, simple negation or negative intent is expressed by an(i), whereas inability or impossibility is expressed by mos.
“Long” forms make the main verb into a dependent verb form (see course reader, bottom of page 72) followed by the negated verb ha-ta ‘do’. In the long form negative, an has contracted to a form that is now listed as a separate lexical entry, anh-ta.
In the table, the verbs are shown in the so-called “present” and “past” tenses with the polite suffix –yo.
Affirmative
|
|
an “short”
|
an “long”
|
mos “short”
|
mos “long”
|
capayo
|
X is catching
|
an capayo
|
capci anhayo
|
mos capayo
|
capci mos hayyo
|
capasseyo
|
X caught
|
an capasseyo
|
capci anhasseyo
|
mos capasseyo
|
capci mos haysseyo
|
pwayo
|
X sees
|
an pwayo
|
poci anhayo
|
mos pwayo
|
poci mos hayyo
|
pwasseyo
|
X saw
|
an pwasseyo
|
poci anhasseyo
|
mos pwasseyo
|
poci mos haysseyo
|
The negative imperative is expressed using the verb mal-ta ‘don’t do’. It works like the long form negatives in terms of the form of the main verb.
capayo
|
catch (it)!
|
|
|
|
|
capci malayo
|
don’t catch (it)!
|
*an capayo
|
*capci anhayo
|
*mos capayo
|
*capci mos hayyo
|
pwayo
|
look!
|
|
|
|
|
poci malayo
|
don’t look!
|
*an pwayo
|
*poci anhayo
|
*mos pwayo
|
*poci mos hayyo
|
another piece of important information: Korean has somewhat optional nominal relational morphemes (course reader, page 72): a case ending -ka that marks subject and a case ending -lul that marks the object. For example,
key-ka thokki-lul capasseyo ‘the dog caught the rabbit’
dog-subject rabbit-object caught
The object-marking –lul, but not the subject-marking -ka can be used in long form negation:
‘X doesn’t catch’ capci-lul anhayo capci-lul mos hayyo
‘X didn’t catch’ capci-lul anhasseyo capci-lul mos haysseyo
*capci-ka anhayo *capci-ka mos hayyo
In the box on the next page, make up a lexical entry (phonological form, category, combinatory potential, and meaning) for the dependent verb form suffix used with negatives.
(b) (4.5 points) More dependent verb forms. Here are some sentence sets, all in the “present” tense, polite form (see the present tense affirmative forms above). The sentence at the top of each column shows the verb used as a main verb, and the examples (i-iv) the same verb in various dependent verb forms.
|
say-lul capayo /cap-a-yo/
bird-object is catching
|
he is catching a bird
|
chek-ul pwayo /po-a-yo/
book-obj he is looking
|
he is looking at the book
|
i.
|
say-lul capki(-lul) cohahayyo
bird-obj catch(-obj) likes
|
he likes to catch/ catching birds
|
chek-ul poki(-lul) sicakhayyo
book-obj look at(-obj) starts
|
he starts to look at/ looking at the book
|
ii.
|
capum-ul kiekhayyo
catching-obj. remembers
|
he remembers catching
|
say-lul pom-ul alayo
bird-obj looking-obj knows
|
he knows [he is] looking at birds
|
iii.
|
say-lul capko (*-lul) sipheyo
bird-obj to catch (*obj) I want
|
I want to catch a bird
|
chek-ul poko (*-lul) isseyo
book-obj looking there is
|
he is looking at the book
|
iv.
|
say-lul capko (*-lul) wasseyo
bird-obj catch came
|
he caught a bird and came
|
say-lul poko (*-lul) kasseyo
bird-obj see went
|
he saw a bird and went
|
note: The impossibility of *-lul in (iii-iv) indicates that these forms must not be nouns.
Here are some ungrammatical sentences corresponding to (i-iv) in the left-hand column. (Assume that parallel modifications in the right-hand column would also be ungrammatical.)
(i) *say-lul capum-ul cohahayyo (iii) *say-lul capki(-lul) sipheyo
*say-lul capko cohahayyo *say-lul capum-ul sipheyo
(ii) *capki-lul kiekhayyo (iv) *say-lul capki(-lul) wasseyo
*capko kiekhayyo *say-lul capum-ul wasseyo
Make up lexical entries (phonological form, category, combinatory potential, and meaning) for the dependent verb form affixes used in (i-iv).
(i) (ii)
(iii-iv)
(c) Further uses of dependent verb forms. Here are some words that use the affixes seen in (i) and (ii) in part (b) above. You will use these to fill in the table in III.
Root
|
|
V+suffix of type (i)
|
Root
|
|
V+suffix of type (ii)
|
po-
|
see
|
poki
|
example
|
ilu-
|
tell
|
ilum
|
name
|
ilu-
|
tell
|
*iluki
|
|
po-
|
see
|
*pom
|
|
ppay-
|
extract
|
ppayki
|
subtraction
|
el-
|
freeze
|
elum
|
ice
|
el-
|
freeze
|
*elki
|
|
ppay-
|
extract
|
*ppaym
|
|
pala-
|
wish
|
hay-palaki
sun-wishing
|
sunflower
|
kel-
|
be fertile
|
kelum
|
fertilizer
|
kel-
|
be fertile
|
*kelki
|
|
pala-
|
wish
|
*palam2
|
|
III. (6 points) Categorizing morphology as inflectional or derivational. On page 82 of the course reader is a table comparing “canonical” inflectional and derivational morphology along several parameters. Very often, morphology does not conform 100% to the canonical types. In the table below, check the box for yes or no for each parameter for the morphologies listed in the left-hand column. (Some parameters in the table in the reader are omitted because they are indeterminant from the data here.)
|
Forms of the same word?
|
Form driven by grammar?
|
Change category?
|
Productive?
|
Compositional?
|
|
Yes
|
No
|
Yes
|
No
|
Yes
|
No
|
Yes
|
No
|
Yes
|
No
|
I.Fula verb morphology
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
I Tamazhaq verb morphology
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
I Bade verb morphology
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
II(a) Korean neg. dependent V
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
II(b) Dep. V form type (i)
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
II(b) Dep. V form type (ii)
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
II(b) Dep. V form type (iii-iv)
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
II(c) Suff. forms of type (i)
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
II(c) Suff. forms of type (ii)
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
IV. (3 points) English negation. As in all the languages described above, English has special constructions for negated verbs. An obvious change is adding not, usually contracted to –n’t, which is cliticized to the preceidng word.
affirmaive
|
negative
|
I have enough money.
|
I do-n’t have enough money.
|
I had enough money.
|
I did-n’t have enough money.
|
Describe the changes that take place in negation in addition to adding not/-n’t? A short phrase should be sufficient to answer these questions.
What syntactic alteration does negation trigger?
What change in tense marking morphology on the verb does negation trigger?
Directory: peoplepeople -> Math 4630/5630 Homework 4 Solutions Problem Solving ippeople -> Handling Indivisibilitiespeople -> San José State University Social Science/Psychology Psych 175, Management Psychology, Section 1, Spring 2014people -> YiChang Shihpeople -> Marios S. Pattichis image and video Processing and Communication Lab (ivpcl)people -> Peoples Voice Café Historypeople -> Sa michelson, 2011: Impact of Sea-Spray on the Atmospheric Surface Layer. Bound. Layer Meteor., 140 ( 3 ), 361-381, doi: 10. 1007/s10546-011-9617-1, issn: Jun-14, ids: 807TW, sep 2011 Bao, jw, cw fairall, sa michelsonpeople -> Curriculum vitae sara a. Michelsonpeople -> Curriculum document state board of education howard n. Lee, Cpeople -> A hurricane track density function and empirical orthogonal function approach to predicting seasonal hurricane activity in the Atlantic Basin Elinor Keith April 17, 2007 Abstract
Share with your friends: |